Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
January 2008: Psyche has a new publisher, Hindawi Publishing, and is accepting submissions

Stanley H. Faeth.
Maternal care in a lace bug, Corythucha hewitti (Hemiptera: Tingidae).
Psyche 96:101-110, 1989.

Full text (searchable PDF, 1476K)
Durable link: http://psyche.entclub.org/96/96-101.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

MATERNAL CARE IN A LACE BUG,
COR YTHUCHA HEWITTI (HEMIPTERA: TINGIDAE)* Department of Zoology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona 85287-1501
Many species in at least 13 families of Hemiptera display parental care (Wilson, 1971). For the Tingidae (lacebugs), a relatively large family with member species that are often very abundant (Drake & Ruhoff, 1965), parental care has been described in detail for only a few species. Tallamy's (1982, 1984, 1985, 1986) and Tallamy & Den- no's (198la, b, 1982) extensive studies of the lace bug, Gargaphia solani, showed that maternal care of offspring is age specific, has a genetic basis, and greatly improves offspring survival by reducing predation. Maternal care, however, is costly in that tending females are less fecund (Tallarny, 1982,; Tallamy & Denno, 1981a, b, 1982). Three other lace bug species do not exhibit maternal care, but two of these are not subjected to intense predation and the third possesses other life history traits to avoid predation (Tallamy & Denno, 1981b).
In this paper, I describe the biology and parental care behaviors for Corythucha hewitti (Drake), a lace bug that has a wide distribu- tion (Canada and northeastern through northcentral US (Drake and Ruhoff, 1965)), high densities, and causes extensive plant dam- age. Neither the biology nor behavior of this species has been previously described. To test the hypothesis that tending by female parents influences persistence of their broods, I experimentally removed female adults.
Material care can evolve and persist if increased inclusive fitness of adult females via increased survival of brood offspring outweighs assumed costs of tending or protecting offspring (Wilson, 1975; Tallamy 1982, 1986; Tallamy & Denno, 1981a, b, 1982). Fitness via increased brood survival may be further increased if kin discrimina- tion occurs, so that related individuals are treated preferentially and *Manuscript received by the editor February 26, 1989. 101
Ps~che %:101-110 (198(1). http:l/psyche cnlcIllb orfflW6-lOI html



================================================================================

102 Psyche [vo~. 96
energy and time are not wasted in caring for non-relatives (Wald- man et al., 1988). For Corythucha hewitti, kin discrimination should be particularly important due to high densities; unrelated broods often co-occur on the same leaves and nymphs and adults probably often encounter unrelated conspecifics when broods move en masse to nearby leaves. In an initial test of kin discrimination, I experimentally removed maternal adults and replaced them with adult females from unrelated broods. I then compared persistence of these experimental broods and the adult females with control broods.
The biology and behavior described here are based on observa- tions and experiments from 14 June-July 2, 1987 and 30 June-July 18, 1988 at University of Minnesota, Lake Itasca Forestry and Bio- logical Station, Lake Itasca, MN. The habitat is a mixed hardwood- conifer forest.
To test the hypothesis that presence of a female adult affects persistence of the brood, I removed adult lace bugs from their broods. Fifty pairs of leaves (a pair occurred on the same twig) on five Prunus virginiana (chokecherry) trees, each with a brood of first or second instars and an adult, were labelled with tape near the petiole. For each pair, an experimental and a control brood was assigned randomly by a coin flip. The adult female was removed from each experimental brood on 4 July 1988. Nymphs in each experimental and control brood were counted. I assumed that the adult present was indeed the mother. Although actual kinships were not determined, this assumption is reasonable since females were observed to remain near their broods from egg through nymphal stages. On July 7 1988, all experimental and control broods were re-censused. Seventeen of 50 pairs were not used in statistical analy- sis (Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing difference in fraction of nymphs remaining) either because one of the leaf pairs was missing or control broods had no adult present at the time of re-censusing. This experiment determines the effect of adult females on persist- ence of the nymphal brood; the actual fate of missing nymphs can- not be ascertained.
To test the hypothesis that kin discrimination or recognition occurs between a mother and her nymphs, I removed adult females and



================================================================================

19891 Faeth - Corythucha hewitti 103
replaced them with unrelated females. I assumed that females removed from broods were indeed mothers and that females used to replace these mothers were unrelated or at least not maternal to the nymphs. Twenty four pairs of leaves on two Betula papyrgera (paper birch) trees harboring at least one adult and a third to fourth instar nymphal brood were marked (as above) and one of each pair assigned randomly as experimental or control. On experimental leaves, the adult female was removed with forceps and replaced with a female from a brood on a different branch on 1 1 July 1988. At the same time, adults on control leaves were lifted from the leaf with forceps but then returned to the same leaf. Lifting was done to control for any effects caused by physically disturbing the females. Nymphs and adults on control and experimental leaves were censused four days later; 20 of 24 experimental leaves and 23 of 24 control leaves were recovered. A Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to statistically compare fraction of remaining nymphs between experimental and control leaves.
Biology and Behavior of the Lace Bug.
During the summers of 1987 and 1988, the lace bug was at high densities (>lo per leaf; N = 1000 haphazardly-selected leaves per tree species) on P. virginiana (common chokecherry), Tilia ameri- cana (basswood or linden), and B. papyrifera (paper birch). The lace bug was also noted on Prunuspennsylvanica (pin cherry), P. serotina (black cherry), and Quercus macrocarpa (bur oak), although less so than the former three tree species. All of these represent new host records as this species has been recorded from only species of Corylus (Drake and Ruhoff, 1965). Some trees were damaged extensively; i.e., most leaves showed evidence of the distinctive dis- coloration and senescence caused by lace bug feeding. Eggs are laid in clusters of usually 20-50 attached to the underside of a leaf and can be found in late June, although by this time most nymphs are in the first instar. Presence of some first instar broods in mid July suggests more than one generation per season. Mean size of first instar broods on chokecherry was 25.2 k 18.0 (range = 5 - 102, n = 100 broods). The larger of these broods, however, may be the result of coalescing broods from different females on the same leaf or from egg dumping (Tallamy, 1985).




================================================================================

104 Psyche [Val. 96
First instar nymphs begin feeding gregariously on the underside of the leaf near the base by puncturing the leaf with their beaks. The attending adult female remains near the leaf edge usually with her head pointed toward the brood (Fig. 1). On several occasions, I observed females that wandered from the leaf with the brood, but these eventually returned to the brood. Nymphs complete five instars in 20-30 days, while typically remaining within the brood. Apparently, broods can complete development on a single large leaf like basswood but may move to other leaves on trees like choke- cherry, where leaves are relatively small. When moving to another leaf, adult females lead the way, rapidly vibrating the abdomen in a vertical plane. It is not known if an aggregation pheromone is involved in movement from leaf to leaf, although such has been suggested for other lace bugs (Kearns & Yamamoto, 198 1). After completion of nymphal stages, eclosed adults tend to remain feeding in broods. In later stages, many broods are observed with more than one adult (range = 0 - 8 adults/ brood, Z = 1.76 + 1.68, n = 100 broods), although I cannot be certain that additional adults are from the same brood. Adults likely overwinter in leaf litter and emerge in early June as do other species in this genus (Bailey, 1% 1). Adult females do not display aggressive behaviors when either they or their nymphs are disturbed (i.e., prodded slightly with a pencil point in the field (n = 20) or in the laboratory on removed twigs (n = 20)). Gargaphia solani, on the other hand, aggressively defends nymphs against predators and inanimate objects (Tallamy & Denno, 1981a). When a nymph of C. hewitti was disturbed, it removed its beak from the plant tissues and began to move toward and then down the midrib, occasionally stopping and vibrating its abdomen in a vertical plane. Other nymphs in the brood followed. The adult female scurried down the leaf and arrested the nymphal column by stopping on the midrib, rapidly vibrating her abdomen and occasionally turning to face the nymphs. In this manner, the nymphs ceased moving and began to feed where the adult had perched. When adult females were disturbed, they moved down the midrib, stopped to vibrate the abdomen, and either stopped on the same leaf or moved to another leaf. Her nymphs usually followed (12 of 20 observations) and settled to feed wherever the adult even- tually stopped. Occasionally, nymphs moved past a female perched on a twig but in 4 of 8 cases, the female re-located the brood on a



================================================================================

19891 Faeth - Corythucha hewitti 1 05




================================================================================

106 Psyche [vo~. 96
leaf within one hour. Fluttering of wings by adults was reported for G. solani and suggested as a predator defense by increasing appar- ent size of lace bugs (Tallamy & Denno, 198la) or as way of direct- ing nymphal movements (Kearns & Yamamoto, 198 1). However, I observed only abdominal vibrations. Furthermore, nymphs with reduced wings performed the same vibrating behavior. It thus appears that maternal care consists of keeping the brood together by leading and "herding" rather than actively defending against natural enemies.
Field Experiments on Brood Persistence
Proportion of nymphs remaining in broods after adult females were removed was significantly less than broods where females were present (control % = .453, SD = .305, n = 33; experimental X = .117, SD = .197, n = 33; Wilcoxon rank sum test, large sample approxi- mation, W* = 5.07, p < .001).
When maternal females were replaced with unrelated adult females, proportion of remaining nymphs was significantly less than proportion remaining on control leaves where adult females were disturbed but not replaced (control Z = -296, SD = .355; experimen- tal 2 = -086, SD = .240; Wilcoxon rank sum test, large sample approximation, W* = 2.29, p < .05). Percent of experimental leaves (mothers replaced with unrelated females) with adults remaining was 12.5% (3 of 20) compared to 39.1% (9 of 23) for control leaves (mothers disturbed but not replaced). During the course of this experiment, a severe thunderstorm occurred and, coupled with physical disturbance of lifting, may account for the low proportion of remaining nymphs and adults on the control leaves, However, the storm should have affected control and experimental broods equally.
The evolution of parental care assumes that the costs of caring (e.g., time and energy investments, risk of predation, reduced fecun- dity) are outweighed by the gains (e.g., increased survival and repro- duction of offspring) (Wilson, 1975; Tallamy & Wood, 1986). Results from this study suggest potential gains in fitness by maternal females. Without the mother, nymphs in the brood disappear, and probably wander to other leaves. This study did not determine their



================================================================================

19891 Faeth - Corythucha hewitti 107
fate, but it is likely that increased movement increases mortality from natural enemies, desiccation, or failure to find a suitable feed- ing site. Further, feeding in groups may enhance survival of nymphs. Aggregated nymphs may be able to use a wider array of leaf types, or groups of nymphs may create nutrient "sinks" (Tal- lamy & Denno, 198la). Additional experiments examining the growth, survival, and reproduction of solitary versus group nymphs of C. hewitti should be productive.
The female replacement experiment suggests that some degree of kin recognition or discrimination (sensu Waldman et al., 1988) occurs. The direction of recognition (whether mothers recognize their nymphs or nymphs recognize mothers or both) is not clear since it was not possible from the maternal replacement experiment to determine if adult females deserted unrelated nymphs or vice- versa. Nevertheless, kin recognition should be advantageous in this system because high densities, multiple broods per leaf, and move- ment of entire broods from leaf to leaf via petioles and stems increase the likelihood of encountering unrelated conspecifics. Other subsocial hemipterans and homopterans display varying degrees of kin recognition. Elasmucha grisea and Antiteuchus trip- terus females do not distinguish between their own eggs and conspe- cifics when guarding them (Melber and Schmidt, 1977; Maschwitz and Gutmann, 1979 cited in Eickwort, 1981), but female Pisilus tipuliformis recognize and prefer to guard their own eggs (Parker, 1965). Membracid females accept and protect eggs of conspecifics, but unlike Corythuca hewitti, are unlikely to encounter conspecifics in nature (Hinton, 1977). In the tingids, female G. solani orient to their own eggs and nymphs more so than unrelated ones but will also guard unrelated eggs (Tallamy, 1985, 1986; Tallamy & Denno, 1981a).
Maternal adults of C. hewitti appear to care for their nymphal brood by maintaining the feeding aggregation through "herding" and leading the brood to new leaves when disturbed or perhaps when the resources of a leaf are depleted. Adult females appear non-aggressive, at least when confronted with an inanimate object. Tallamy and Denno (198la) found, however, that live predators and inanimate objects produced defensive responses in the tingid G. solani. Despite the lack of aggressiveness of C. hewitti, female par- ents could indirectly protect nymphs by maintaining the integrity of



================================================================================

108 Psyche [vo~. 96
the brood if rate of attack by natural enemies is less on individuals in groups or if increased movement by nymphs attracts natural enemies. Further, female parents may indirectly protect nymphs by releasing alarm pheromones that cause nymphs to scatter in the presence of predators as Kearns & Yamamoto (1981) suggested for G. solani. Althogh I observed no predation or parasitism of nymphs, I did observe egg broods that were heavily parasitized by wasps and I cannot exclude the possibility that mothers actively protect eggs or other life stages.
Adult female G. solani care for eggs and nymphs by actively defending them against predators (Tallamy, 1982,1984, 1985,1986; Tallamy & Denno 198la, b). Gargaphia solani feeds mostly on horsenettle in old fields where predation is intense and probably a strong selective force in maintaining maternal care (Tallamy & Denno, I98 1 a, b). Tallamy & Denno (198 1 b) showed that predation was less severe on two Corythucha species that feed on forest trees, and these species did not display parental care. Corythuca hewitti feeds in similar forest habitats as do these species (in one case, on the same host tree) and thus predation may not be severe and ma- ternal care is instead maintained by other selective pressures. Adult female lace bugs (Corythucha hewitti) remain in close prox- imity to their broods from egg stage, through five nymphal instars, to adulthood. Such subsocial is relatively common in the Hemiptera but few have examined experimentally the nature and consequences of such behavior. I tested the hypotheses that presence of mothers affects the persistence of the nymphal brood and that the presence of the mother as compared to an unrelated female promotes persist- ence of nymphal broods. In broods where mothers were removed, brood size was significantly less than control broods. When unre- lated females were exchanged with mothers, brood size and number of adults also declined relative to controls. These results suggest that the presence of mothers increases persistence and probably survival of offspring nymphs and that either nymphs recognize their moth- ers, or vice-versa. Behavioral observations suggest that mothers do not actively protect offspring, but rather keep broods intact by "herding" or leading nymphs to new feeding sites.



================================================================================

19891 Faeth - Corythucha hewitti 109
I thank the faculty and staff, particularly Jon Ross, resident biol- ogist, of the University of Minnesota, Lake Itasca Forestry and Biological Station for their generous support in facilities, supplies, and advice. Without the company and field assistance of Graham and Thad Faeth, the study may have been possible but certainly less enjoyable. Consultation and advice from Michael Auerbach and John Alcock greatly aided this research. Comments from J. Alcock, S. Rissing, and R. Rutowski improved the manuscript. D. Rubbelke provided the photograph. R. C. Froeschner (Smithsonian Institu- tion) kindly identified the lace bug specimens. BAILEY, N. S.
195 1. The Tingoidea of New England and their biology. Ent. Amer. 31: 1 - 140. EICKWORT, G. C.
1981.
Presocial insects. In: Social Insects, Vol. 2, ed. H. R. Hermann, Aca- demic Press, New York. pp. 199-281.
DRAKE, C. J. & F. A. RUHOFF
1965.
Lacebugs of the world: a catalog (Hemiptera: Tingidae). Smithsonian Institution, US National Museum, Bull. 243, Washington, DC. HINTON, H. E.
1977.
Subsocial behaviour and biology of some Mexican membracid bugs. Ecol. Entomol. 2: 61-79.
KEARNS, R. S. & R. T. YAMAMOTO
1981. Maternal behavior and alarm response in the eggplant lace bug, Garga- phia solani Heidemann (Tingidae: Heteroptera). Psyche 88: 215-23. MASCHWITZ, U., & C. GUTMANN
1979.
Spur- und Alarmstoffe bei der gefleckten Brutwanze Elasmucha grisea. Insectes SOC. 26: 101-1 11.
MELBER, A. & G. H. SCHMIDT
1977.
Sozialphanomene bei Heteropteren. Zoologica (Stuttgart) 127: 19-53. PARKER, A. H.
1965. The maternal behaviour of Pisilus tipuliformis Fabricius (Hemiptera: Reduviidae). Entomol. Exp. Appl. 8: 13-19. TALLAMY, D. W.
1982. Age specific maternal defense in Gargaphia solani (Hemiptera: Tingi- dae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 7-1 1. 1984. The ecology of insect parental care. Bioscience 34: 20-24. 1985. 'Egg dumping' in lace bugs (Gargaphia solani, Hemiptera: Tingidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 17: 357-362.
1986. Age specificity of 'egg dumping' in Gargaphia solani (Herniptera: Tingi- dae). Anim. Behav. 34: 599-603.




================================================================================

110 Psyche [vo~. 96
TALLAMY, D. W., & R. F. DENNO
198la. Maternal care in Gargaphia solani (Hemiptera: Tingidae). Anim. Behav. 29: 771-778.
1981 b. Alternative life history patterns in risky environments: an example from lacebugs (Hemiptera: Tingidae). In: Insect Life History Patterns: Geo- graphic and Habitat Variation, ed. R. F. Denno & H. Dingle, Springer- Verlag, New York, pp. 129-147.
1982.
Life history trade-offs in Gargaphia solani (Hemiptera: Tingidae): the cost of reproduction. Ecol. 63: 616-620. TALLAMY, D. W. & T. K. WOOD
1986.
Convergence patterns in subsocial insects. Ann. REV. Entomol. 31: 369-390.
WALDMAN, B., P. C. FRUMHOFF, & P. W. SHERMAN 1988.
Problems of kin recognition. Trends Ecol. Evol. 3: 8-13. WILSON, E. 0.
197 1.
The Insect Societies, Belknap Press, Cambridge, Mass. 1975.
Sociobiology. The New Synthesis. Belknap Press, Cambridge, Mass.



================================================================================


Volume 96 table of contents