Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
January 2008: Psyche has a new publisher, Hindawi Publishing, and is accepting submissions

James F. Hare.
A new method for marking ant larvae.
Psyche 95:299-308, 1988.

Full text (searchable PDF, 664K)
Durable link: http://psyche.entclub.org/95/95-299.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

A NEW METHOD FOR MARKING ANT LARVAE
Erindale College, University of Toronto
Mississauga, Ontario, Canada L5L 1C6
In most ethological studies, an observer must recognize individual animals or at least certain classes of animals in order to acquire meaningful information. Individual animals are distinguished either by morphological differences or through the application of a distin- guishing mark by the investigator.
In recent studies conducted by Hare (1987) and Alloway & Hare (under editorial review) where brood of a slave-making ant species (Harpagoxenus americanus) and host species (Leptothorax longi- spinosus) were offered simultaneously to host species workers, an observer was required to identify the species of any given larva quickly and reliably. Species identification of ant larvae has tradi- tionally relied upon one or more of the following morphological characteristics: general body shape, mandible shape, pattern, struc- ture and abundance of hairs, location and abundance of integumen- tary spinules, head shape, dentition of the mandibles and physical characteristics of other mouthparts (see Wheeler & Wheeler, 1960). None of these characteristics are useful in distinguishing Harpa- goxenus from Leptothorax larvae (Wheeler & Wheeler, 1960; Hare, personal observation). Thus for our research, we required a tech- nique whereby larvae could be given a visible mark that would not bias the acceptance of marked versus unmarked larvae. Several techniques have been developed for marking adult ants (see Stuart 1986 for a review). Techniques reported for marking brood either damage the brood (e.g. filling eviscerated larval skins with coloured gelatine; Brian 1975), or the marks do not persist because of grooming by adult workers [e.g. spots of of coloured wax applied to larval cuticle (Brian 1975), bits of coloured plastic glued 'Present Address: Department of Zoology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2E9
Manuscript received by the editor November 20, 1988. Pu&e 95:29S1.307 (1988). http:llpsyche cnlclub orefl5195-299 html



================================================================================

300 Psyche [VOI. 95
to the larval cuticle (Isingrini et al. 1985) or coloured spots of paint applied to cocoons (R. J. Stuart, personal communication)]. Other techniques, although effective, are not efficient in terms of the time required to obtain marked larvae. For example, Brian & Rigby (1978) and Cole (1981) report that Sudan black, an oil soluble dye, fed to a queen results in the production of coloured eggs. Appar- ently, this mark is retained through all life stages including adult- hood (R. J. Stuart, personal communication). For the purposes of our experiments, waiting for dye to be ingested by the queen and for the marked eggs to develop into larvae was inefficient. Adult ants are known to devour even slightly damaged brood rapidly and to regurgitate this material to healthy larvae (Wilson, 197 1). Thus it seems plausible that if a coloured substance is applied externally to brood that causes workers to perceive marked brood as damaged, the coloured substance will end up inside healthy lar- vae in the colony. Such a mark would be visible to a human observer through the translucent larval cuticle, and if effective would provide a rapid and inexpensive means of marking large numbers of larvae. Here I describe a technique for obtaining marked larvae, present data on the persistence of the mark, and demonstrate that marking does not bias larva acceptance by Lepto- thorax ambiguus or L. longispinosus workers. METHODS AND MATERIALS
Nests of both L. ambiguus and L. longispinosus were collected during September, October and November 1985 and from April through July 1986 at various sites within the regional municipalities of Halton and Peel, Ontario, Canada. Ant colonies were cultured in the laboratory employing techniques described by Alloway (1979) and Hare (1987).
In a pilot study, 10 larvae smeared with Testor's red enamel paint (#1150, commonly used to paint plastic models and available at local hobby retailers) were placed inside the nest of each of 17 L. longispinosus colonies. Within 24 h, 5 to 10 (8.5 k 1.87 [mean& SE]) of those larvae were cannibalized by adult workers. During this 24 h period, trophallactic exchange of food from workers to previously unmarked larvae resulted in a large proportion (0.63 k0.17 [mean å SE] ) of each colony's larvae becoming marked internally



================================================================================

19881 Hare - Marking ant larvae 30 1
with the paint. Between 31.5% and 85.7% of the previously un- marked larvae became marked internally, thus providing a minimum of 6 and a maximum of 34 marked larvae in the test colonies. The colour red was selected for use in all experiments because ants are not sensitive to light of this wavelength (Wilson, 1971), thus reducing the possibility of any visually mediated bias in larva acceptance.
To test for any influence of the mark on larva acceptance, an experiment was performed. Internally marked larvae were obtained by covering the mouthparts and at least 50% of the cuticle of several L. ambiguus larvae with Testor's red enamel and placing these lar- vae inside an L. longispinosus nest. Adults of the recipient colony cannibalized these larvae and in most cases fed this material to their own larvae. Allospecific larvae were used to 'transmit' the mark since they are less likely to be accepted for tending (see Hare and Alloway, 1987) and are thus more likely to be cannibalized. On the next day, worker groups were established by choosing three L. longispinosus workers arbitrarily from each of 13 colonies and placing each group in a new culture dish with a nest, water vial and food (see Hare and Alloway 1987). Groups were allowed 24 hours to explore their new environment prior to testing. Since Plateaux (1960) found that Leptothorax nylanderi workers could only distinguish between conspecific larvae and Solenopsis fugax larvae when conspecific larvae were present, larvae of the two 'types' were offered simultaneously to the workers. Under this pro- tocol, a single larva of each 'type' (marked and unmarked) were placed on a clean glass coverslip and positioned immediately in front of the nest entrance. Within each pair, larvae were matched visually for size and placed as close together as possible, (but not in physical contact) in the centre of the coverslip. Workers were allowed to retrieve one larva into their nest, their choice was recorded, and a new pair of larvae was presented. Paired presenta- tions of this sort continued until the supply of either marked or unmarked larvae for a given donor colony was exhausted. For the 13 worker groups (replicates) the number of initial paired presenta- tions ranged from one to 12 (8.2å±0. presentations [meankSE], mode = 10 presentations). Since each worker group was derived from a separate maternal colony, replicates were independent and the difference in the number of the two types of larvae accepted in



================================================================================

302 Psyche [vo~. 95
paired presentations across groups can be analysed using the Wil- coxon paired sample test (Zar 1974). Occasionally more than one ant was involved in the retrieval of larvae, and in some instances both larvae were judged to be taken simultaneously. For the pur- pose of analysis those ties were omitted and the number of presenta- tions for that group consequently reduced. Following the series of paired presentations, marked and un- marked larvae which had not been taken were re-introduced in one large pile on a glass coverslip outside the nest entrance. The amount of time taken to retrieve the remaining larvae was recorded (if time allowed on the day of the trial) and the number of each type of larva remaining in the nest was recorded daily until the internal mark could no longer be distinguished (6 to 16 days). For each day, data were analysed using a heterogeneity Chi-square test (Zar 1974) that compared the observed number of each type of larva in the nest tc an expected null difference within each group (i.e. if no preference were exhibited then we would expect equal numbers of the two types inside the nest) and for the pooled difference across groups. The experiment was replicated using 16 L. ambiguus worker groups and larvae from their own maternal colonies. For L. ambi- guus, the number of initial paired presentations ranged between five and 17 (9.0+ 1.1 days [mean* SE]). Data from these presentations are included and analysed as outlined above. However, the marked larvae used in these trials were marked seven to 10 days (7.7d10.9 [mean* SE]) in advance of the trials (i.e. these were marked for a pilot study addressing the feasibility of obtaining marked larvae). Consequently the record of the number of marked and unmarked larvae remaining in the nest became increasingly confounded over time as the internal mark faded or disappeared. This trend became noticeable on the third day of observations and thus for L. ambi- guus data are only presented on acceptance over time for the first two days.
Reliable data on the persistence of the internal mark are available only for the larvae offered to L. longispinosus workers in the "mark bias" experiment. In those trials, the mark duration ranged from six to 16 days (1 1.1 k 1.0 days [mean* SE], mode = 12 days) across groups.




================================================================================

19881 Hare - Marking ant larvae 303
In the series of simultaneous presentations neither L. ambiguus nor L. longispinosus worker groups showed a significant preference for retrieving marked or unmarked larvae (Tz32.5, P>0.50 and T = 37, P> 0.50 for L. ambiguus and L. longispinosus respectively). An average of 4.7k0.77 (meankl SE) marked and 4.3k0.52 unmarked larvae were retrieved first by the 16 L. ambiguus groups while 4.3 kO.49 marked and 3.6k0.51 unmarked larvae were re- trieved first by the 13 L. longispinosus worker groups (Fig. 1, day 0). Furthermore, the number of groups accepting a greater number of marked larvae; unmarked larvae, or equivalent numbers of both types in paired presentations (Table I) was similar across the two species (3 X 2 contingency table analysis (Zar 1974), x2= 1.1659, P>0.25).
For both L. ambiguus and L. longispinosus, the data from indi- vidual worker groups were homogeneous on each day of observa- tion and no significant preference for either marked or unmarked larvae was shown by any single group over the course of the study (all P>.25, see Appendix B-1 in Hare, 1987). Similarly, no signifi- cant differences in the number of marked and unmarked larvae remaining in the nests of either species were detected when data were pooled by day (all P> SO, see Appendix B-1 in Hare, 1987). The mean numbers of marked and unmarked larvae accepted by worker groups on any given day were similar, although at almost all time intervals the average number of marked larvae per nest was slightly higher than that for unmarked larvae (Fig. 1). The large increase in the mean number of marked and unmarked larvae accepted for groups of both species between day 0 and day 1 reflects the fact that the day 0 data include only those larvae retrieved first in the series of paired presentations. Following this series of presentations, 'non-preferred' larvae were re-introduced and overall, 99.3% of the marked larvae and 97.2% of the unmarked larvae available to the L. ambiguus worker groups were accepted by day 1 while 98.3% of the marked larvae and 100% of the unmarked larvae available were accepted by L. longispinosus worker groups during the same period. Thus, even those larvae that were 'non- preferred' in a simultaneous choice situation were acceptable to Leptothorax workers.
The increase in the mean number of marked larvae in the L. longispinosus nests between days 14 and 15 (Fig. 1) is a result of the



================================================================================

304 Psyche
L. ambiquua (N=16)
L. longispinosus
IVol. 95
Figure 1: Mean number of marked larvae (solid circles) and unmarked larvae (open circles) accepted by Leptothorax worker groups over time. [Note Day 0 = number accepted upon termination of paired presentations].



================================================================================

19881 Hare - Marking ant larvae 305
Table 1.
Initial preference profiles of Leptothorax worker groups. Worker Species
L. ambiguus L. longispinosus
Groups accepting more marked larvae 7 8
Groups accepting more unmarked larvae 6 4 Groups accepting equal numbers of
both types of larvae 3 1
reduced sample size and does not reflect a change in the number of larvae in the nests of the remaining groups (see Appendix B-1 in Hare, 1987). For the two L. longispinosus groups that were observed on the sixteenth day, 95% of the marked larvae and 80% of the unmarked larvae that were initially available remained intact within the nest. Without exception, marked and unmarked larvae were placed in a common brood pile within the nests of both species throughout the experiment.
The introduction of larvae marked externally with Testor's paint provides an inexpensive and reliable technique through which large numbers of internally marked larvae can be obtained rapidly (<24h). Fading of the mark was perceivable in some groups as early as six days after marking, but remained visible in others to a maxi- mum of sixteen days. Thus, this technique would not be useful in situations requiring an extended mark duration, but its utility would ultimately depend on the specific rate of trophallactic food exchange of the ants in question. The technique should also be limited to use with small worker groups since high rates of trophal- laxis between larvae and workers in full colonies can lead to con- tamination of unmarked larvae with the mark (Hare, unpublished data). However, this would not present a problem over the short term since "contaminated" larvae possess a very faint mark relative to those introduced as "marked" larvae in the first place (Hare, personal observation). For the purposes of our experiments with Leptothorax, the mark duration was more than adequate and there were no instances of contamination in any trial using groups of three workers.
No significant preference for marked or unmarked larvae occurred at any time interval and overall neither L. ambiguus nor L.



================================================================================

306 Psyche [vo~. 95
longispinosus showed a preference on any day for marked or unmarked larvae. Furthermore, ants placed marked and unmarked larvae in a common brood pile within the nest. Thus the presence or absence of the mark itself did not influence larva-acceptance by Leptothorax.
Throughout the course of the experiment, deviations from equiv- alent numbers of marked and unmarked larvae accepted typically were in the direction of a greater number of marked larvae within both species. However, those deviations were quite small. The explanation for this pattern lies in the observed tendency of Lepto- thorax workers to prefer large larvae (Hare 1987). Whenever larvae were marked, it appeared that large larvae became marked more readily than small larvae. This in itself suggests that large larvae receive preferential treatment (in terms of regurgitation from adults) in their maternal colonies and results in uneven size distributions of marked and unmarked larvae. In spite of efforts to match larvae as closely as possible on the basis of size, marked larvae were often slightly larger than the unmarked larvae used, thus explaining any apparent trend towards preferential acceptance of marked larvae. A technique is described whereby a visually observable mark can be transmitted to the gut contents of ant larvae. The technique is inexpensive, provides large numbers of marked larvae on demand, and is useful in situations requiring a mark duration of less than 10 days. Results of a choice experiment demonstrate that the mark itself does not bias larva acceptance by Leptothorax workers. Funding for this research was provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada in the form of a grant to T. M. Alloway and a post-graduate scholarship to J. F. Hare. I thank Lew Holowaty, Andrew Kajioka, Alikeshs Leigh1 and Roger Mosquera for technical assistance and T. M. Alloway and R. J. Stuart for enlightening discussions during the development of this technique. I also thank Karl Larsen, R. B. MacWhirter, J. 0. Murie and P. J. Young for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript.




================================================================================

Hare - Marking ant larvae
ALLOWAY, T. M.
1979. Raiding behaviour of two species of slave-making ants, Harpagoxenus americanus (Emery) and Leptothorax duloticus Wesson (Hyme- noptera: Formicidae). Animal Behaviour, 27: 202-210. ALLOWAY, T. M. & HARE, J. F. (under editorial review in Behaviour). Experience independent attraction to slave-maker ant larvae in host-species ant workers (Leptothorax longispinosus Hymenoptera: Formicidae). BRIAN, M. V. 1975. Larval recognition by workers of the ant Myrmica. Animal Behaviour, 23: 745-756.
BRIAN, M. V. & RIGBY, C.
1978. The trophic eggs of Myrmica rubra L. Insectes Sociaux, 25: 89- 1 10.
COLE, B. J.
1981. Dominance hierarchies in Leptothorax ants. Science, 212: 83-84.
HARE, J. F. 1987. Brood discrimination by leptothoracine ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). M. Sc. Thesis, University of Toronto. HARE & ALLOWAY.
1987. Early learning and brood discrimination in leptothora- cine ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Animal Behaviour 35: 1720- 1724. ISINGRINI, M., LENOIR, A. & JAISSON, P. 1985. Preimaginal learning as a basis of colony-brood recognition in the ant Cataglyphis cursor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 82: 8545-8547. PLATEAUX, C.
1960. Adoptions expferimentales de lams entre des fourmis de genres diffkrents I: Leptothorax nylanderi Forster et Solenopsis fugax Latreille. Insectes Sociaux, 7: 163- 170.
STUART, R. J.
1986. Use of polyester fibers to mark small leptothoracine ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society, 59: 566-568.
WHEELER, G. C. & WHEELER, J.
1960. The ant larvae of the subfamily Myrmici- nae. Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 53: 98-1 10. WILSON, E. 0. 1971. The Insect Societies. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The Bel- knap Press of the Harvard University Press. 548 pp. ZAR, J. H.
1974. Biostatistical Analysis. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice- Hall. 620 pp.




================================================================================


Volume 95 table of contents