Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
January 2008: Psyche has a new publisher, Hindawi Publishing, and is accepting submissions

Bert Hölldobler.
Chemical communication in Meranoplus (Hymenoptera: Formicidae).
Psyche 95:139-152, 1988.

Full text (searchable PDF, 3456K)
Durable link: http://psyche.entclub.org/95/95-139.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

PSYCHE
Vol. 95 1988 NO. 3-4
CHEMICAL COMMUNICATION IN MERANOPLUS
(HYMENOPTERA: FORMICIDAE)*
Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology Harvard University
Cambridge, Mass. 02138, U.S.A.
The chemical communication systems of social insects, and in particular of ants, has been intensively studied by many research groups during the past 25 years. These studies included the identifi- cation and description of exocrine glands involved in pheromone production (see Holldobler and Engel, 1978; Holldobler, 1982; Jessen et a1 1979; Billen, 1987), the investigation of the natural product chemistry of pheromones (see Blum, 1982; Morgan, 1984; Attygalle and Morgan, 1985), and the study of the underlying behavioral mechanisms and evolution of chemical communication (see Holldobler, 1978; 1984; Buschinger and Maschwitz, 1984). Nevertheless, there are some groups of ants that have been almost completely neglected by behavioral biologists in spite of the fact that they are relatively abundant and probably ecologically important. One such group is the genus Meranoplus, which is very common in Australia. In this paper I report the results of my experimental studies of the communication behavior of several Australian Mera- noplus species.
*Manuscript received by the editor October 27, 1988. 139




================================================================================

Psyche [vo~. 95
The work was conducted in the laboratories of the Division of Entomology of CSIRO in Canberra, Australia, during a research year in 1980. Colonies of Meranoplus were collected near Canberra (ACT), near Poochera, South Australia, and near Lake Eacham and Eungella, Queensland. A total of seven species were investi- gated. They are listed in table 1 with my acquisition number and the species number of the Australian National Insect Collection (ANIC)/ (assigned by R. W. Taylor), where voucher specimens have been deposited.
Colonies or groups of workers with brood have been housed in moist gypsum nests, or in test tubes containing water held in by a cotton plug, and fed honeywater, freshly killed cockroaches and a specially prepared diet (Bhatkar and Whitcomb, 1970). The nest of each colony was placed in plastic boxes (30 X 75 X 10 cm) which served as foraging arenas.
Glandular dissections were performed in distilled water on ants killed by placing them for a few minutes in a freezer. Additional information concerning procedures will be given with the descrip- tion of the individual experiments.
Recruitment to nests and food
The most detailed results were obtained with Meranoplus sp. no. 11. Two colonies of this species'were collected in the Corin Dam area, near Canberra. Each of the colonies had one queen, approxi- mately 200 workers and larvae. In addition, in the same area, I collected two queenless worker groups each containing approxi- Table 1.
Species of Meranoplus investigated in this study. Acquisition ANIC
Species number number Locality
Meranoplus hirsutus 135 Near Lake Eacham, Qld 177 Eungella, Qld
Meranoplus sp.
3 2 11 Near Canberra, ACT
Meranoplus sp.
70 11 Near Canberra, ACT
Meranoplus sp.
150 12 Near Monga Forest, NSW
Meranoplus sp.
194 13 Near Poochera, SA
Meranoplus sp.
195 14 Near Poochera, SA
Meranoplus sp.
246 15 Near Canberra, ACT




================================================================================

19881 Hotldobler - Chemical communication 141 rnately 50-60 workers. Although the queenless worker groups were found at least 50 m away from both queenright colonies, when they wer joined with a queenright colony, they readily merged without any sign of aggression or aversion.
The first indication that Meranoplus communicates by chemical trails was obtained when I dumped a field-collected colony inside the arena where I also provided a dark, moist test-tube nest. The colony first gathered in clusters along the arena wall. A few workers explored the surroundings of the arena, until after 43 minutes the first scout discovered the test tube nest. A few minutes later 3 more workers entered the nest. When leaving the nest the workers clearly exhibited trailing behavior, tapping their gaster tips repeatedly to the gound and sometimes even dragging them on the floor for dis- tances of 0.5-1.0 crn (Fig. I). This behavior was especially conspicu- ous near the entrance of the nest tube. Subsequently, the number of workers moving toward the nest increased markedly, although no clear trail following could yet be noticed. However, after an addi- tional period of approximately 30 minutes, a continuous trail, lead- ing from the cluster of ants to the nest, began to be established (Fig. 2a). Scouts, which returned from the newly discovered dark nest, Figure I. A worker of Meranoplus hirsutus dragging its abdominal tip over the ground during trai! marking. The arrow points to the extruded sting.



================================================================================

142 Psyche [vo~. 95
often exhibited a vigorous shaking behavior when encountering nestmates. This seemed to stimulate the nest mate to follow the trail. The shaking behavior was especially strong and persistent when recruiting workers encountered the queen. Often 3 to 6 workers approached the queen simultaneously, alternately gnawing with their mandibles on her cuticle or vigorously shaking their bodies toward the queen. This behavior was primarily aimed at the queen's gaster as if the workers were attempting to push the queen forward. At the beginning the queen moved very haphazardly, but once she contacted the trail, she walked straight to the nest. There was no pulling or dragging behavior, but occasionally I observed adult transport in the typical myrmicine mode, with the carried ant curled over the back of the carrier. The carried ant assumes the "pupal" position, with antennae and legs tightly folded to the body. Mera- noplus workers often take this same position when mechanically disturbed. In addition I have seen them inside the nest in this posi- tion, lying on their sides as if they were sleeping. Once the colony had settled inside the new nest I replaced the paper cover of the arena floor and offered honey water in the arena in approximately 15 cm distance from the nest entrance. As soon as the first forager discovered the food source it imbibed the liquid and returned to the nest. In order to observe the colony inside the nest I removed the aluminum foil cover from the nest tube. The ants did not show any sign of disturbance. The returning scout exhibited a vigorous shaking behavior .when encountering nestmates. This behavior did not noticeably differ from that performed during col- ony movement. The scout engaged in frequent oral trophallactic exchanges, sometimes with 2 to 3 nestmates simultaneously. This behavior was frequently interrupted by renewed shaking behavior. When the scout finally left the nest again to return to the food source, it repeatedly dragged its abdominal tip on the ground for short intervals. The scout was usually followed by 5-15 ants (Fig. 2b). When encountering nestmates in the arena, the scout also dis- played the shaking which clearly appeared to stimulate the nest- mates to follow along the trail to the food source. Occasionally I even observed recruiters moving repeatedly back and forth on the trail without even entering the nest, vigorously shaking their bodies whenever they encountered nestmates. This behavior resulted in a relatively quick establishment of a trail between food source and



================================================================================

19881 HoVdobler - Chemical commwamcatlOn 143 Figure 2. Trail following behavior in Meranoplw. a. During colony movements the ants move along a well established route to the nest provided in the test tube (covered with an aluminum foil). b. A trail laying recruiter moves from the ne~t to the food source and is followed by 6 nestmatcs.



================================================================================

144 Psyche [vo~. 95
nest, because in this initial phase almost each worker returning from the feeding site to the nest appeared to lay a trail. The anatomical sources of the trailpheromones The fact that recruiting Meranoplus workers frequently tap with their abdominal tip the ground, suggests that either one of the major sting glands (Poison gland and/or Dufour's gland) or the rectal bladder are involved in trail communication. Each of the three
organs were dissected out of freshly killed workers, crushed on hardwood applicators and rubbed along a line of 25 cm length. Each test trail was offered to the ants simultaneously with a control trail, which consisted either of water, or of the secretions of one of the other organs. Both trails originated in the same circle of 0.5 cm in diameter directly at the nest entrance, but diverged in an angle of 40'. The sides of control and test trails were changed in irregular sequence.
All Meranoplus species investigated have unusually large Dufour's glands. In the species Meranoplus sp. 11 the Dufour's gland stretches almost the whole length of the gaster, whereas the poison gland is relatively small, with a pair of bulging glandular filaments (Fig. 3). In a series of pilot tests it quickly became apparent that secretions from both the poison and Dufour's glands elicit trail following behavior in Meranoplus workers, whereas the contents of the rectal bladder had no effect as a trail substance. There was, however, a significant difference between the ants' responses toward the two trail substances: Trails drawn with crushed poison glands elicited a much stronger initial response than those drawn with Dufour's glands. The poison gland trails, however, were only effec- tive for a few minutes, whereas Dufour's gland trails had an orient- ing effect for at least 7 hours and possibly even longer. Table 2 shows the quantitative results of the trail tests. It is interesting to note that considerably more ants followed the Dufour's gland trail when it was presented simultaneously with a poison gland trail, probably due to the stimulating effect of the poison gland secretions (independent samples t-test: p = 0.067). This is further supported by the following results.
Dufour's gland secretions alone did not elicit a substantial trail following response in Meranoplus workers. But when a crushed poison gland on the tip of an applicator stick was offered simul- taneously at the nest entrance, a significant number of ants emerged



================================================================================

Holldobler - Chemical cornrnunicat ion
145
Figure 3.
Schematical illustration of the outline of the gaster of a Meranoplus worker showing the large Dufour's gland (D) and the poison gland (P) with its bulging filaments.
from the nest and followed the Dufour's gland trail. From these experiments one can conclude that both the poison and the Dufour's gland serve in chemical trail communication. The poison gland secretions appear to function as relatively short lived recruitment signals. The Dufour's gland secretions do not elicit a strong recruit- ment response, but they appear to be much longer lasting, and motivated ants follow a Dufour's gland trail. This conclusion was further supported by the following experiments: Meranoplus workers were stimulated to leave the nest along a 5 cm long trail drawn with a freshly crushed poison gland. After 5 cm this trail was continued with a trail drawn 60 minutes previously with Dufour's gland secretions. Almost 83% (24) of the ants arriv- ing at the transition point continued to follow the Dufour's gland trail through at least 10 cm. Additional observations in the labora- tory and in the field confirm that Meranoplus establish trails which are very long lasting so that they function as trunk routes. From my



================================================================================

146 Psyche [vo~. 95
Table 2.
Number of workers of Meranoplus sp, no. 1 1 following artificial trails drawn with crushed poison and Dufour's glands. All ants following the trail at least through 10 cm during a 2 minute observation period were counted. The means and standard deviations of a total of 6 experiments for each combination are given. Poison gland vs water Dufour's gland vs water Poison gland vs Dufour's gland
experimental results I conclude that these long lasting routes are marked with Dufour's gland secretions.
Finally, I could not find evidence that the trail pheromones of Meranoplus are species specific. Meranoplus sp. no. 11 readily responded to poison gland trails of Meranoplus hirsutus. When glandular secretions of both species were offered simultaneosuly in a choice test, there was no preference for the conspecific poison or Dufour's gland trails. Meranoplus sp. no. 11 followed the poison gland and Dufour's gland trails of Meranoplus spp. no. 12, 13, 14, and 15. Similarly, Meranoplus sp. no. 14 followed trails drawn with poison or Dufour's gland secretions of M. hirsutus, M. spp no. 11 and 14.
Repellent secretions in the Dufot.uk glands Most Meranoplus species studied became motionless and assumed a pupal position when they were mechanically disturbed. In con- trast, M. hirsutus workers arched their gasters upright and extruded a whitish, sticky substance at the tip of the exposed sting (Fig. 4a). The same defense behavior could be observed when M. hirsutus foragers encountered other ant species, especially at a food source (Fig. 4b). The secretions, which originate from the Dufour's gland, elicit a strong repellent effect in other ant species such as Campono- tus consobrinus, Polyrhachis sp. and Rhytidoponera sp. I was unable to test whether the Dufour's gland secretions of the other Meranoplus species studied have a similar defensive function in interspecific interactions.
Nest site marking
In the laboratory nests of Meranoplus sp. no. 11 I noticed that within a few days the paper floor of the foraging arena was marked with fecal droplets. These markings increased in density near the nest and were especially conspicuous directly at the nest entrance (Fig. 5a).




================================================================================

19881 Holldobier - Chemical communication 147 Figure 4. Meranoplus hirsutus workers in defensive posture. a. Workers which were mechanically disturbed, immediately arch up their gaster and extrude a droplet of Dufour's gland secretions on the exposed sting, b. The same behavior can be observed in workers guarding a piece of prey.



================================================================================

148 Psyche pol. 95
Figure 5. Nest area making in Merunopfus. a. The fecal spots are especially dense near the nest entrance. b. On a marked paper the Meranaphis workers gather directly at the spot where the nest entrance of a previously removed nest was located.



================================================================================

19881 Holldobler - Chemical communication 149 To investigate whether these markings can serve as homing cues, the paper floor was placed into another arena and 20 ants, taken from the nest tube, were released in the new arena. In all five repli- cas of the same test, most of the displaced ants had gathered within 15 minutes at the exact spot on the paper floor where the nest entrance was previously located (Fig. 5b). In a second series of tests the colony Meranoplus sp. no. 11 was transferred into a new arena with a new paper floor and two pieces of paper (5 X 5 cm) were placed approximately 5 cm from the nest entrance. The centers of both papers were approximately 10 cm apart. Papers marked by the test colony, by a conspecific foreign colony, by a colony of another Meranoplus species and unmarked papers were tested in different combinations (Tab. 3). Approxi- mately 40 ants were released into the arena, and subsequently the nest tube was removed. After an adjustment period of 15 to 20 minutes, I counted the number of ants on each paper. At least one hour elapsed before another test was conducted and the left-right arrangement of the pairs of test papers was alternated from test to test to take into account possible artifactual preferences for one side or another. As can be seen from table 3 workers of Meranoplus sp. no. 1 1 appear to be able to differentiate the markings from their own colony from those of conspecific foreign colonies and colonies of other Meranoplus species. Interestingly, when encountering for- eign markings the ants did not exhibit a particular aggressive behav- ior or avoidance. This suggests that the markings do not function as territorial signposts, but rather as homing and colony recognition cues.
Table 3. Number of ants of the test colony (Meranoplus sp. no. 11) counted on papers marked by different Meranoplus colonies. The means of 7 experiments and standard deviations are given.
Test colony vs unmarked paper
6.57 * 2.51
0.71 * 0.95
Test colony vs foreign conspecific colony 9.00 * 2.94
1.43 * 1.27
Test colony vs colony of Meranoplus hirsutus 8.14 k 3.72
1.43 * 2.15




================================================================================

Psyche
[Vol. 95
The communication behavior of seven Australian species of the genus Meranoplus was studied in the laboratory. All species exhib- ited basically the same recruitment communication during nest movements and recruitment to food sources. Scouts stimulate nest- mates by a vigorous body shaking behavior. They lay short lasting recruitment trails with secretions from the poison gland. Long last- ing trunk routes are apparently marked with relatively non-volatile secretions from the Dufour's glands. All Meranoplus species studied have very large Dufour's glands, stretching the whole length of the gaster. In comparison the poison gland is small. In at least one species the secretions of the Dufour's gland also function as defen- sive secretions against enemies and predators. The nest area of Meranoplus is marked with colony specific chemical cues, which are probably contained in the fecal droplets. Many thanks to N. F, Carlin, who read the manuscript, and to R. W. Taylor for identifying the ants. I am most grateful to R. W. Taylor and the Division of Entomology, CSIRO, Canberra (Australia) for their generous hospitality. This work was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation, the National Geographic Society and a fellowship from the John Simon Grggenheim Foundation.
ATTYGALLE, A. B. AND E. D. MORGAN.
1985.
Ant trail pheromones. Advances in
Insect Physiology, 18: 1-30.
BILLEN, J. 1987.
Morphology and ultrastructure of exocrine glands in social Hymenoptera. In J. Eder and H. Rembold, ed. Chemistry and Biology of Social Insects. (Tenth International Congress of the International Union for the Study of Social Insects, August 19861, pp. 81-84, J. Peperny, Munich. BLUM, M.S. 1982. Pheromonal bases of insect sociality: communications, conun- drums and caveats. Les Mediateurs Chimiques (Les Colloques de l'INRA, Ver- sailles, 1981). 7: 149-162.
BHATKAR, A. AND W. H. WHITCOMB.
1970.
Artificial diet for rearing various
species of ants. Fla. Entomologist 53: 229-232. BUSCHINGER, A. AND U. M ASCHWITZ.
1984. Defensive behavior and defensive
mechanisms in ants. In H. R. Hermann, ed., Defensive mechanisms in social insects, pp. 95-150. Praeger, New York.




================================================================================

19881 Holldobler - Chemical communication 15 1 H~LLDOBLER, B., 1978. Ethological aspects of chemical communication in ants. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 8: 75-1 15. H~LLDOBLER, B. 1982. Chemical communication in ants: new exocrine glands and their behavioral function. In M. D. Breed, C. D. Michener, and H. E. Evans, ed., The biology of social insects. (Proceedings of the Ninth Congress of the International Union for the Study of Social Insects, Boulder, Colorado, August 1982) pp. 312-317. Westview Press, Boulder. H~LLDOBLER, B. 1984. Evolution of insect communication. In T. Lewis, ed. Insect Communication (Twelfth Symposium of the Royal Entomological Society, London) pp. 349-377. Academic Press, London. H~LLDOBLER, B. AND H. ENGEL. 1978. Tergal and sternal glands in ants. Psyche, 85: 285-330.
JESSEN, K. U., U. MASCHWITZ, AND M. HAHN. 1979. Neue Abdominaldrusen bei
Ameisen. I: Ponerini (Formicidae: Ponerinae). Zoomorphologie, 94: 49-66. MORGAN, E. D.
1984. Chemical words and phrases in the language of phero- mones for foraging and recruitment. In T. Lewis, ed., Insect communication (Twelfth Symposium of the Royal Entomological Society, London), pp. 169- 194. Academic Press, London.




================================================================================


Volume 95 table of contents