Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
January 2008: Psyche has a new publisher, Hindawi Publishing, and is accepting submissions

Jennifer C. Cornell, Nancy E. Stamp, and M. Deane Bowers.
Variation and developmental change in activity of gregarious caterpillars, Hemileuca lucina (Saturniidae).
Psyche 95:45-58, 1988.

Full text (searchable PDF, 952K)
Durable link: http://psyche.entclub.org/95/95-045.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

VARIATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGE IN
ACTIVITY OF GREGARIOUS CATERPILLARS,
HEMILEUCA LUCZNA (SATURNIIDAE)
Variation in behavior of individuals within an aggregation may result in a nominal division of labor or roles (Wilson, 1971). Indi- viduals may play a particular role throughout the course of devel- opment or change roles often. Such variation seems to occur in tent caterpillars of the genus Malacosoma (Lasiocampidae) (M. califor- nicum pluviale, Wellington, 1957; M. disstria, Laux, 1962 (as M. neustria), Greenblatt and Witter, 1976; M. americanum, Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982). Evidence for permanent polyethism (in terms of active or inactive individuals) was reported for At. c. pluviale by Wellington (1957) but was not found by Myers (1978), and Welling- ton's statistical analysis has been criticized (Papaj and Rausher, 1983). In other tent caterpillar species, individuals were not consist- ently active or inactive but shifted from one to the other state over a few days (Laux 1962; Greenblatt and Witter, 1976; Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982). Nonetheless, those larvae that were currently active played a critical role in determining the foraging pattern of the aggregation. For instance, small subgroups of M. americanum larvae left the tent before the main body of caterpillars, fed, and returned, depositing a fresh recruitment trail that the other larvae then followed to the feeding site (Fitzgerald, 1980). To date, only Malacosoma species have been investigated for behavioral variation among larvae in aggregations even though many gregarious species exhibit distinct foraging patterns where individuals of various activity levels might be important. Further- more, none of the Malacosoma studies assessed activity instar by VviMuseum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
department of Biological Sciences, State University of New York, Binghamton, NY 13901, USA
Manuscript received by the editor February 24, 1988. Pu&e 95:45-58 (1988). hup Ytpsychu einclub orgWi95-045 html



================================================================================

46 Psyche [vo~. 95
instar, which might be expected to change as mobility increases with larval size and as availability of food and presence of predators changes through the larval season. Larvae of the buckmoth, Hemi- leuca lucina Hy. Edw. (Saturniidae), provide an excellent example for comparison with tent-making species. H. lucina larvae lay silk trails, probably with a chemical marker, as do H. oliviae (Capinera, 1980) and move with tandem trail-following. H. lucina larvae often defoliate portions of the hostplant, Spiraea latifolia (Ait.) Borkh. (Rosaceae) and the aggregations are then forced to move to new feeding sites. Even newly-hatched larvae move substantial distances relative to their size to find food (Bowers and Stamp, unpubl. data). Larvae also may move distances of 3 m or more for thermoregula- tion, when they are attacked by predators and parasitoids, and when they molt (Bowers and Stamp, unpubl. data). Transient polyethism in some tent caterpillars (e.g. M. america- num) may be associated with central-place foraging, web mainte- nance and group cohesion (Greenblatt and Witter, 1976; Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982). In contrast, H. lucina larvae would be pre- dicted to exhibit patterns of individual behavior different from Malacosoma species because H. lucina larvae are nomadic foragers, do not construct webs and exhibit a declining tendency to aggregate over development that occurs sooner than in Malacosoma species. Our objective was to examine individual variation in activity, using distance traveled as an index, in larvae of H. lucina. Specifi- cally, we determined: 1) whether larvae were consistently slow, fast or intermediate travelers relative to the group when tested several times within an instar, 2) whether patterns of larval activity (as measured by relative distance traveled) were consistent across instars, and 3) whether larval activity was correlated with larval weight.
Four egg masses of H. lucina were collected from a population in Dover, Massachusetts. The groups were reared separately on S. latifolia in petri dishes. During the first through third instars, larvae were kept in a growth chamber at 25OC day: 20å¡ night, with a photoperiod of 16 h day: 8 h night. For the remaining instars, larvae were reared in the laboratory at 25OC in large plastic containers (46 X 30 X 7 cm) to prevent overcrowding.




================================================================================

19881 Cornell, Stamp, & Bowers-Hemileuca lucina 47 Each larva was individually marked using a three-dot, color- coding system with seven colors of acrylic paint (LiquitexTM) and weighed 4 days after each molt. Reliable identification of individu- als was maintained by isolating individuals just before molt, remark- ing them after molt and then returning them to their groups, within 12 hr of separation from the group. Thus, larvae could be identified individually throughout their development and during testing, while remaining with their sibling groups. In contrast, other studies have isolated individuals for substantial periods (Greenblatt and Witter, 1976; Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982), which may alter larval behavior.
During the first through fifth instars, larvae from each group were tested for distance traveled as an index of activity. We did not test the last (sixth) instar because those larvae are not gregarious. Lar- vae were tested at 4 days after molting and were starved for 24 h prior to testing. The test arena was a petri dish (diameter 14 cm) lined with a sheet of paper on which were drawn a series of concen- tric circles, 2.5 mm apart. A paper rope was suspended vertically by a string so that the bottom touched the center of the dish. The ropes were made from paper towels soaked in 50% ethanol and twisted into 300-mm lengths. When dry, the ropes were marked in pencil at 2.5 mm intervals. These ropes simulated the natural form of a branch along which larvae forage. A clean rope was used for each test. A sprig of S. latifolia was placed at the top of the string and a heat lamp was placed 1 m above that to induce the larvae to travel up the rope.
At the beginning of the test, the larvae were arranged on the paper sheet in a ring around the base of the rope. Larvae faced towards the rope and were 5 cm from it. They then were allowed to move around the dish or up the rope for 8 min for the first through third instars and for 4 min for the fourth and fifth instars. The reduction in test time was necessary due to an increase in overall larval activity in the later instars. The rank order of all larvae on the rope and the dis- tance each had traveled from the starting point were recorded at the midpoint and end of each trial.
Each group of larvae was tested in 5 trials during each instar. Because individual larvae could be identified throughout the tests, we were able to obtain data on behavior of individuals at several levels: 1) variation within a trial (i.e. did larvae change ranks during



================================================================================

48 Psyche [vo~. 95
the course of a trial?); 2) variation from one trial to another within an instar (i.e. were larval ranks consistent from one trial to another?); and 3) variation from one instar to the next. One-tailed Spearman rank correlation was used to assess the degree of changing ranks within trials by larvae (i.e. due to turning around or possibly passing one another) with 1 indicating perfect agreement between ranks (i.e. no change) and 0 indicating no agreement.
To examine whether individual larvae were consistently ranked the same with regard to distance traveled, over the five trials within an instar, we used Kendall's coefficient of concordance (W) (Con- over, 1080; Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982). If individuals were con- sistently ranked the same among trials, W = 1.0, whereas if rank varied randomly from trial to trial, W = 0. To determine whether distance traveled by individual larvae was consistent from one instar to the next, larvae were ranked on the basis of total distance traveled in 5 trials per instar and Spearman's rank correlation (rs) was used.
Frequency distributions of distances traveled by individuals within an aggregation tend to exhibit a graded series (or hierarchy), in which relatively few individuals travel far and many individuals travel relatively short distances over a short test period (e.g. Fitzger- aid, 1980; Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982). Following the terminol- ogy and definitions of Weiner and Solbrig (1984), we believe that the analytical methods used by plant ecologists to evaluate a graded series of sizes of individuals within a population are particularly appropriate for behavioral data such as distance traveled when: 1) the aggregation yields large variation in individual distances, 2) there are relatively few large distances and many small ones, and 3) the few large distances contribute greatly to the observed pattern. Weiner and Solbrig (1984) indicate that standard measurements of skewness are inappropriate because they are designed to be insensi- tive to the degree of variability and only reflect the second aspect of the three listed above. Specifically in our study, it is inequality among larvae in distance traveled, not asymmetry in the distribution of distances ((2) above), that is of biological interest here. Therefore, the variation in distance traveled was represented graphically using Lorenz curves and evaluated using the Gini statistic (Weiner and Solbrig, 1984). Gini coefficients take into



================================================================================

19881 Cornell, Stamp, & Bowers-Hemileuca lucina 49 account all three characteristics listed above. Individuals were ranked according to total distance traveled (over 5 trials) and the cumulative percent of distance traveled plotted against the cumu- lative percent of the individuals per group. If all larvae traveled the same distance, a plot of this would yield a diagonal line from the origin to upper right (e.g. a Lorenz curve with a Gini coefficient = 0). The degree of deviation from the diagonal is a measure of inequality and is evaluated by the Gini coefficient, where 0 indicates all indi- viduals are equal in distance traveled and a maximum of 1 repre- sents complete inequality (i.e. all individuals but one having a value of 0; Weiner and Solbrig 1984).
Thus, when inequalities in distance traveled were present, the Lorenz curves and Gini coefficients provided a means to assess the degree to which the inequalities change from instar to instar and relative proportion of individuals responsible for these inequalities. For example, a high Gini value would indicate that a few individuals traveled either a small distance or quite far relative to the others and those two possibilities can be distinguished by the shape of the curve.
Individuals usually did not change ranks after half way through a trial (Table I), although a few individuals turned around or passed others. For half the tests (9 of 19), ranks of individuals, as deter- mined by distance traveled, were stable from trial to trial (Kendall's coefficient of concordance, Table 2). Total distance traveled in each instar was not correlated consistently with larval weight (Table 3) and therefore we concluded that distance traveled was unrelated to larval size.
Total distance traveled by each larva was compared between instars to determine whether those inequalities in distance traveled within an instar persisted into the next one. In most instances, total distance traveled in one instar was not correlated with distance traveled in the previous instar (Table 4). When a significant correla- tion occurred, it was between the third and fourth instars and between the fourth and fifth instars.
The pattern of considerable variation in distance traveled among larvae is illustrated graphically by the Lorenz curves, which show the inequalities in total distance traveled in 5 trials by individuals in



================================================================================

50 Psyche [vo~. 95
Table 1.
Comparison of ranks of individuals in groups after half the test period and at the end of the test, with Spearman rank correlation (rs). Significance level (P) for one tailed test with a = 0.05 is shown. In this case, 1 indicates no change in ranks by individuals between the half way point and the end of the test period, and 0 represents no agreement between those ranks. In the cases below, a significant corre- lation indicates agreement between ranks of individuals half way through the test and their ranks at the end. Dashes represent no movement by any individuals in that trial. First instar larvae of Group D did not move so no correlation coefficient could be calculated.
Group Instar n rs P
A I 50 0.641
<O.OO 1 *
2
45 0.821 <O.OO l*
3 43 0.559 <0.001*
4 3 6 0.739
<O.OO 1 *
5 28 0.577 <0.001*
1 50 0.771
<O.OO 1 *
2 41 0.888 <O.OO 1 *
3 40 0.842 <0.001*
4 37 0.574 <O.OO 1 *
5 3 5 0.760 <O.OO 1 *
1 48
0.876 <O.OO 1 *
2 4 1
0.851 <O.OO 1 *
3 38 0.686 <0.001*
4 39 0.933 <O.OO 1 *
5 25 0.476 <0.006*
1 50 -
-
2 34 0.767 <O.OO 1 *
3 35
0.833 <0.001*
4 33 0.750
<O.OO 1 *
5 26
0.488 <0.006*
*Indicates statistical significance.
groups (Fig. 1). In the first and second instars, each group had many individuals that either did not move or only traveled short distances and a few individuals that moved relatively long distances (Fig. 1 A, B, C). In contrast, in the third through fifth instars, inequalities still occurred but with a few individuals traveling quite short distances and a few traveling far.
But one group (D) exhibited a different pattern than the others. In 5 trials, none of the first instar larvae moved and thus there was no Gini coefficient, and in contrast to the other groups, only a few second instar larvae remained stationary (Table 5; Fig. ID).



================================================================================

19881 Cornell, Stamp, & Bo wen- Hemileuca kina 51 Table 2.
Comparison of distance traveled among 5 trials, with n = number of larvae, W = Kendall's coefficient of concordance, and significance (P) < 0.05 indi- cated by *. The null hypothesis for the Kendall test was W = 0 when distances varied randomly from trial to trial. Thus, a significantly high value indicates that distances traveled by individuals were consistent from trial to trial. Group Instar n W P
A 1 50 0.307 >O. 10
2 45 0.330 >0.05
3 43 0.458 <0.001*
4
36 0.245 >0.50
5
28 0.327 >0.10
1 50
0.197 >0.50
2 41 0.289 2.20
3 40 0.479 <O.OO 1 *
4 37
0.370 <0.05*
5 35
0.715 <O.OO 1 *
C 1
48 0.402 <O.O 1 *
2
41 0.334 X.05
3
38 0.338 X.05
4
39 0.326 >0.05
5 25 0.599
<O.OO 1 *
D 1 50 - -
2 34
0.190 >0.50
3 35
0.609 <O.OO 1 *
4 33
0.464 <O.O 1 *
5 26
0.428 <0.05*
Because it was common for larvae to remain massed for several hours at a time, we could not justify ignoring that group (D) in our statistical analysis on the grounds that it was abnormal. But conse- quently, we could not apply the appropriate statistical test, a nonpa- rametric two-way ANOVA which requires values in all cells, to the Gini coefficients, and a parametric test was inappropriate because it is unclear what assumptions can be made about the underlying distribution of Gini coefficients (Weiner and Solbrig, 1984). There- fore, we analyzed only instars 11-V. The Gini coefficients were sig- nificantly different among instars 11-V (Friedman's test, followed by multiple comparisons (Conover, 1980); Friedman x2 = 8.400, df = 3, P = 0.04; Table 5, Fig. 1). Thus, inequalities in distances traveled decreased from instar to instar.




================================================================================

Psyche [vo~. 95
Table 3. Relationship of larval weight and total distance traveled by individuals in groups. Spearman rank coefficients (rs) and probability (P) at a = 0.05 are indicated.
Group Instar n rs P
-
*Indicates statistical significance
DISCUSSION
In general, studies on Malacosoma species (Laux, 1962; Green- blatt and Witter, 1976; Edgerly and Fitzgerald, 1982) and Hemileuca lucina reveal only transient leaders and followers, at most. For instance, Edgerly and Fitzgerald (1982) found a significant behav- ioral difference among M. americanum larvae in only 3 of 8 colo- nies, and they found no correlation in activity levels across trials. In contrast to that study, we examined larval behavior more directly by following marked individuals within trials, among trials and across instars, and found that in some cases individual H. lucina were consistently traveling relatively long distances across trials and instars. But no pattern emerged that would allow us to predict which individuals might lead or in which instar that might occur. For example, Group C had two instars in which rankings among trials were stable (i.e. some individuals were consistently traveling faster than others), showed a significant positive correlation



================================================================================

19881 Cornell, Stamp, & Bowers- Hemileuca lucina 53 Table 4.
Relationship between instars for total distance traveled by marked individuals in groups. Spearman rank coefficients (rs) and probability (P) at a = 0.05 are indicated.
Group Instar n rs P
A 1-2 42 0.227 0.15
2-3 34 0.218 0.22
3-4 27 0.308 0.12
4-5 20 0.097 0.69
D 1-2 - - -
2-3 25 0.369 0.07
3-4 23 0.446
0.03*
4-5 2 1 0.303
0.19
*Indicates statistical significance
between larval weight and distance traveled for Instars 11-V, and exhibited a significant correlation between distance traveled between instars for two such combinations. In contrast, Group A had only one instar in which rankings were stable among trials and one instar in which distance traveled was correlated with weight. In H. lucina, transient polyethism occurred at several levels: 1) Over the course of a trial, larval ranks did not change; thus, leaders during a single foraging bout (represented here by a single trial) remained at the forefront. 2) In some cases, ranks of individual larvae were constant from one trial to the next within an instar. The instar in which this was most prevalent was the third (for 3 or 4 groups). However, in other cases, there was no consistency in larval ranking from trial to trial. Thus, in some groups the same indi- viduals led a series of foraging bouts, whereas in others, the leaders varied from bout to bout. In some instances, a significant positive correlation occurred between larval weight and distance traveled, but in other cases, there was no correlation. Thus, within a foraging group, the largest larvae were sometimes the leaders, sometimes randomly distributed within the traveling group, but were never consistently the slowest (i.e. none of the negative correlation coeffi-



================================================================================

54
100
LU
0
z
2
(A
0
50
h-
-1
3
2
3
0
a"
100
!A
u
z
h-
It)
LU
> 50
h-
_J
3
2
3
0
a?
Psyche
IVol. 95
50 100
"/I. CUMULATIVE INDIVIDUALS
100
% CUMULATIVE INDIVIDUALS
Figure 1. Pattern of inequality in distance traveled by individual larvae in sibling groups. Numbers adjacent to curves indicate the instar tested. Groups A, B, C and D are shown. First instar larvae of Group D were tested, but they remained aggregated and thus are not shown.
cients in Table 3 were significant). 3) In a quarter of the tests, ranks of individual larvae from one instar to the next remained consistent. This was primarily between the third and fourth instars (for 3 of 4 comparisons).
The Lorenz curves and Gini coefficients showed that the patterns of distance traveled by individuals changed over instars. In many cases, especially in the early instars, a few individuals moved great distances whereas others hardly moved. By the later instars, varia- tion was still large but the inequalities (or skewness) among individ- uals were much less.




================================================================================

19881 Cornell, Stamp, & Bowers-Hemileuca lucina 55 Table 5.
Patterns of larval variability in total distance traveled over 5 trials by larvae in groups. Gini coefficients (unbiased estimator, G') are shown, with 0 repre- senting complete equality and 1 indicating total inequality among larvae. First instar larvae of group D were tested but remained aggregated. Instar
-- -
Group 1 2 3 4 5
A 0.569 0.612 0.380 0.365 0.282
B 0.640 0.543 0.486 0.40 1 0.418
C 0.532 0.583 0.310 0.450 0.342
D - 0.405
0.279 0.272 0.2 12
The changes in larval activity of H. lucina revealed by the Lorenz curves and Gini coefficients parallel other developmental changes we have observed in H. lucina. These larvae exhibit distinct changes in behaviors between the early and late instars. While they have strong aggregation tendencies through the third instar and remain in groups in the fourth and fifth instars unless disturbed, the tendency to aggregate declines steadily and sixth (last) instar larvae are soli- tary (Cornell, et al., 1987). H. lucina larvae also have a repertoire of defensive and escape behaviors that changes from one instar to the


Volume 95 table of contents