Kevin M. O'Neill.
Territorial Behavior in Males of Philanthus psyche (Hymenoptera, Sphecidae).
Psyche 86:19-44, 1979.
Full text (searchable PDF, 1688K)
Durable link: http://psyche.entclub.org/86/86-019.html
The following unprocessed text is extracted from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.
TERRITORIAL BEHAVIOR IN MALES OF
PHILANTHUS PSYCHE (HYMENOPTERA, SPHECIDAE)* BY KEVIN M. O'NEILL
Department of Zoology and Entomology
Colorado State University
Fort Collins, Colorado 80523
The comparative ethology of the family Sphecidae has been the subject of intensive study over the last thirty years, although the behavior of these wasps was observed and recorded by Fabre as early as the mid-1800's. The majority of work has been done on females, probably because of the conspicuousness of their activities and their complex, species-specific behavior patterns. In the past few years, an increasing amount of work has concentrated on the behavior of male sphecids, revealing complex and sometimes puz- zling behavior patterns. Lin's paper (1963) on male territoriality in Sphecius speciosus was one of the first attempts at a rigorous study of male behavior. The conspicuousness of female activity is often matched by the inconspicuousness of the males, particularly when they do not frequent the nesting area subsequent to emergence. Even when males are easily observed, in the nesting area or elsewhere, matings are rarely observed. Male sphecids, except in a few species, do not participate in the nesting activities of the females.
Alcock et al. (1978) classify the mating strategies of male acu- leate Hymenoptera as taking the form of either searching for females (e.g., the "sun dance" of Nyssonine sand wasps, Evans 1966) or waiting for (e.g., territoriality) females at particular localities where they are most likely to be found. The distribution of receptive females in time and space has been a major influence on the evolution of male mating strategies. A recent review of territorial behavior in male sphecids (Alcock 1975a) shows that in all known cases this behavior is associated with securing females for copula- tion. I define territoriality here as "a fixed area from which intruders are excluded by some combination of advertisement, threat or *Manuscript received by the editor March 27, 1979, 19
Pzirfif WxI9.44 (1979). hup Yftwcht rnlclub orgt8WW-Ol9 html
================================================================================
20 Psyche [March
attack" (Brown 1975). Since territoriality is the exception rather than the rule in male sphecids, some ecological factors must impinge, favoring the evolution of the behavior in some species and not in others. According to Alcock, territoriality seems to evolve when males can locate areas where females are clumped or occur predictably. In addition, these areas must be defendable in terms of the time and energy budgets of the males. Lacking these conditions the large time and energy expense associated with defending a territory may not maximize reproductive success. Recently particular attention has been paid to male and female behavior in the sphecid genus Philanthus. In most North American species in which male behavior has been observed males exhibit territoriality. They also exhibit a specific behavior referred to as abdomen dragging which has been suggested to function in scent marking the territories with a mandibular gland secretion (Gwynne 1978) which may attract females (Alcock 1975a). Philanthus psyche Dunning is a relatively small member of its genus, females and males averaging 9 and 8 mm in length, respec- tively. It is found in western and central North America (Great Plains and Great Basin) occupying sand dunes with low sparse vegetation, particularly grasses. Nesting areas are usually on the periphery of blowouts or similar dunal features. Observations on Philanthus psyche were made about two miles east of Roggen, Weld Co., Colorado during July and August 1976 and 1977 and occasionally throughout the summer of 1978. The general study area may be classified as sand sagebrush (Harrington 1964). The specific study site was the area immediately surrounding a vegetation-free blowout, one side of which was bordered by a sandy ridge. The nesting area of P. psyche was in the border of the blowout and was level to slightly sloping. It was dominated by blowout grass [Redfieldia flexuosa (Thurb,) Vasey] and lanceleaf scurfpea (Psoralea lanceolata Pursh). Further away the dominant plants were sunflower (Helianthus spp.) along with various grasses. At the main observation area measuring 12 by 8 meters, detailed observations were made of male and female behavior and of locations of marked males, occupied territories and female nests. Individual males were marked with one or two spots of enamel paint
================================================================================
O'Neill - Behavior of Philanthus psyche
on the thorax. Periodic observations were made of other areas where there were territorial males to compare male behavior and territory and nest distribution, and to check for movement of marked males between the areas.
Wind speed measurements were made with a Datametrics Airflow multimeter Model 800 VTP anemometer. Males and females for dissection were preserved in alcoholic Bouin's solution which maintains the turgidity of the glandular tissue (Humason 1967). Daily Activity Patterns
At Roggen, males and females emerge in late June, the males preceding the females by a few days to a week. Both become active daily at about 1000 and remain so until between 1400 and 1600 hours. Females spend the night in the nest while males sleep in short (6 cm) sleeping burrows which they dig. After emergence from these burrows each morning, males perch on the low vegetation in the nesting area, grooming and occasionally changing perches but not inte.racting with other insects, even digging females. Males begin setting up territories, on warm clear days, between 1030 and 1100. For seven different clear days during the summer of 1977, twelve territories in the main observation area were censused every 15 minutes during the observation period. Only 22% of the territories observed at 1100 were occupied. However, from 1130 until 1300 approximately 50% were occupied on the average. The number thereafter declined, few males being present by 1600. On warm clear days at the peak of seasonal activity, nearly 100% of the territories were occupied during the afternoon hours; on overcast days, few males were territorial.
Location and Characteristics of Territories Both nests and territories occurred in areas of generally short, sparse vegetation rather than in nearby areas of relatively high and dense vegetation or open sand (Figure 1). The mean distance from a nest to the center of the nearest territory was only 35 cm (N = 29; S.D. 17.4; see Figure 2). Where both P. psyche and P. albopilosus occur in the same habitat, the former nests in the peripheral areas around the blowout which contain plants, while the latter nests in the bare, fine-grained sand in the central portion of the dune. This is the situation at Roggen (Evans 1975). The open sand poses restric-
================================================================================
Psyche
[March
OPEN SAND
Figure 1. Map of main observation area showing vegetation, nests and territories. tions to nesting. For species such as P. albopilosus which nest in the open sand, it may be adaptive to dig much deeper nests because shifting sands can disrupt the cells in the nest. Mean cell depth for P. psyche and P. albopilosus at Roggen are 21.5 and 52.0 cm, respectively (Evans 1975 and unpublished data). The ability of females of P. psyche to nest in the peripheral area with stabilizing vegetation allows them to dig shallower nests and possibly more per season and to avoid the high surface temperatures of the open sand during the middle of the day.
On the other hand, areas of more dense vegetation may restrict nesting due to the fact that plant roots interfere with digging. Therefore, although plants stabilize the dune, females require at least a small amount of space between plants for digging. Nests occurred in small bare spots in the vegetation at least a short distance from the base of the plants. Female P. psyche consistently nest in this type of situation, not only in the Roggen area (where the species is extremely abundant), but also in other areas of Colorado and New Mexico where I have observed this species. It was evident that males set up territories in small plots devoid of vegetation (or nearly so) within the nesting area (Figure 2). By
================================================================================
19791 O'Neill - Behavior of PhiZanthus psyche 23 recording the location of plants on which males "scent marked" around the territory and perching points on the sand and marking off the area which they encompassed, a rough measure of territory size can be made. These areas visibly coincide with the bare areas of sand occupied by males in the field. (If we chose, instead, to take the boundary of the territory as the maximum distance from its center at which a male will sight and approach a passing insect, the above measurement would be only a slight underestimate of functional territory size.) The mean size for twelve territories was 0.17 m2 Figure 2.
Map of 3 m2 area in which most observations were made in 1977. Dotted lines indicate the area encompassed by all marked plants and/ or perches of male(s). For several territories it was necessary to record, on two separate days, plants marked by different males. Solid circles and ovals indicate plants that were marked by males. N indicates a nest entrance.
================================================================================
24 Psyche [March
(range 0.10 to 0.25 m2). It was not apparent that territory size changed through the season as has been shown for the territorial bee Hoplitus anthocopoides (Eickwort 1977).
Patterns of Territory Occupation
Both casual observation and censusing showed that certain territories were occupied more frequently than others. Three of the twelve territories censused (A, K and D in Figure 2) were occupied at least 85% of the time in which at least one of the territories in the plot contained a territorial male. Territory D was occupied during 95% of the period. These territories and several others were usually occupied during peak activity periods. Others contained males only sporadically, often for a few consecutive days followed by a lull of a week or more. Furthermore on overcast days and during the latter part of the season, when few males were territorial, males were nearly always observed on the most "popular" territories. In contrast to these, five territories censused (B, E, F, G and H) were each occupied less than 30% of the time. For eleven of these territories, there was information on both size and frequency of occupation. Comparison of these two parameters shows no appreciable relationship between size and quality of the territory, the latter determined by the frequency of occupation (r = 0.3 16; regression not significant). Therefore, the size of the territory (through the given range) or at least of the vegetation-free perch area does not seem to be a factor in choosing a territory, although there is probably a minimum acceptable space that males will utilize.
For certain males that were watched continuously or were marked with paint, it was possible to record approximately how long they occupied a certain territory on a given day. The mean duration of occupation of a territory was 80 minutes (N = 25; S.D. = 45 minutes).
During the summer of 1976, the distribution of territories occu- pied by individual males was recorded within observational limits. Probably due to the difficulty of marking these small wasps without killing them or hindering their movement, only 13 males were seen subsequent to the day on which they were marked, with only six being observed two or more times. The maximum time period over which a marked male was seen was fourteen days and the maximum
================================================================================
19791
O'Neill - Behavior of Philanthus psyche
number of territories a marked male was seen to occupy was nine. Fifty-two territories were recorded for males returning after the day on which they were marked. Of these, forty-six (88%) were in the area of grass in which they were originally marked (area in Figure 1). Only six (12%) were recorded outside this area with only one being more than 5 m away. These six were found during periodic checks of nearby areas (within 50 m) where there were known to be territories and it is possible that others were missed because of the area to be covered.
Intraspecific and Interspecific Interactions Much of the male's time while on the perch is occupied by interaction with intruding insects, particularly conspecifics. An intruder is defined as an insect that flies into or through a territory (or immediately adjacent to it). A male's response towards an intruder may fall into one of two functional categories: aggression towards a conspecific male or investigation of a potential mate. Barash (1977) designates aggression as an interaction between individuals with the result that one must relinquish access to some resource important to its fitness. It is difficult in this species to distinguish between these two functional responses. However, ob- servations support the fact that at least one type of male-male interaction (the swirling flight) represents aggression, suggesting that P. psyche exhibits true territorial behavior. The swirling flight consists of two (or rarely three) males repeatedly circling one another in loops with a radius of 5 to 15 cm, at a speed making them difficult to follow. These flights usually last from less than 1 to approximately 10 seconds. The term was used by Alcock (1975a) to refer to a similar behavior in males of P. multimaculatus. Swirling flights take place between males holding adjacent territories or between territorial and non-territorial males, the latter probably trying to usurp the former's area. Since they take place repeatedly between the same two males over a short period of time, it is unlikely that this behavior could be continued mutual investigation of a potential mate. Swirling flights do not resemble any observed male-female interaction, and are usually terminated by the intruder leaving the immediate area. However, on three occasions at least, it was observed that the continual harassment by the intruder resulted in its usurping possession of the resident's territory. I assume that
================================================================================
26 Psyche [March
this may have been a relatively common occurrence, since territories often changed hands (noticed during periodic scans of the terri- tories). Non-contact aggressive behavior is known for the males of several other species of insects: conocephaline grasshoppers (Morris 197 I), praying mantids (Edmunds 1967), cockroaches (Breed 1972), the sphecid Eucerceris flavocinctus (Steiner 1978) and nymphalid butterflies (Baker 1972). Baker reported that intruding males in the species Inachis io may usurp another male's territory without physical contact, using a behavior similar to the swirling flight. One other type of interaction seems to be a form of aggression at first glance. A territorial male will drop from a height of approxi- mately 10 to 30 cm onto the back of a male perched on the ground. The perched individual may be within the male's territory or an adjacent territory and does not necessarily have to move to elicit the "attack." The striking male seemingly discontinues the encounter himself immediately leaving after a grapple of one second or less. Alcock (1975a) refers to this as "strike and flee" in P. multimacu- latus. A similar behavior was observed when a male hovered above and dropped onto the back of a female working at her nest entrance or one that had landed within his territory. As with male-male grapples, the striking male orients head-t o-head with the female when landing upon her. This is the posture necessary to initiate copulation. For these interactions the period of grappling was longer (several seconds) and was terminated when the female
managed to break free of the male's grasp. Occasionally two males were seen simultaneously grappling a female in this manner. Upon breaking free, the female continued working on the nest or flew away a short distance, perching on the sand for a short period of time before returning. The latter behavior is similar to parasite avoidance noted for this and other species of Philanthus (Evans 1970) and may function to reduce the number of energetically expensive encounters with males. The encounter may be repeated and may result in the female losing her prey. The similarity of this behavior ("strike and flee") towards both males and females sug- gests that the motivation behind it is the same. In other words, the male is pouncing upon a potential mate and terminates the inter- action himself when some proximate cue signals that it is not a conspecific female. This has been suggested as an explanation for a similar form of male-male interaction in the western cicada-killer wasp, Sphecius grandis (Alcock 1975b). Therefore, the swirling
================================================================================
O'NeiZZ - Behavior of Philanthus psyche
INTRUDER
Philanthus psyche Q
P. psyche Q
P. psyche Q w/prey
Bembecinus nanus
Eumenid wasp
Microbembex spp.
Bombyliid fly
Pprnopes edwardsii
Bembix pruinosa
Butterflies
Podal: nia spp.
Ammophila spp.
Mutillid wasp
Pompilid wasp
Asilid fly
Mean distance intruder pursued by males (cm) Figure 3.
Intensity of response (distance pursued) of male P. psyche for each species of intruder. Intruder species are on the horizontal axis, arranged from left to right in order of decreasing similarity to a conspecific female (see Table 1). flight seems to be the only distinctive aggressive male-male inter- action in P. psyche.
A frequent response of a male to an intruder involved approach and/or pursuit of an insect passing through or near the territory. Different insects were pursued different distances by the territorial males, some simply being approached with the male not leaving his territory. Others were pursued relatively long distances. The ques- tion arose that if there is a relationship between the distance pursued and the identity of the intruder, is the intensity of the male's response (distance pursued) based upon the similarity of the in- truder to male or to female conspecifics (i.e., is the response aggressive or an attempt at securing a mate, respectively)? In order
================================================================================
28 Psyche [March
to answer this, I measured the distance pursued by a male when possible to identify the intruder. Distance pursued refers only to those encounters in which males chased an insect that flew straight through or adjacent to the territory. It does not include swirling flights in which the intruder's response would complicate measure- ment of the resident's response. If the response was a simple instantaneous approach, the encounter was given an a priori value of 20 cm since this approximates the radius of the territories. The male did not make contact with the intruder in mid-air as occurs in other territorial sphecids (Lin 1963, Alcock 1975a, Gwynne 1978) except in a few cases where conspecific females were grappled at the end of a flight. For two hundred forty-seven responses to intruders of thirteen different species (minimum of seven per species), I was able to record the pursuit distance. Many more encounters were observed, but could not be recorded either because the distance or the identity of the intruder could not be accurately determined. The responses to conspecifics were differentiated into female, male, and female with prey. Gwynne (1978) has carried out a similar analysis with P. bicinctus using distinct levels of behavior (approach, butt, grapple and grapple to ground, in increasing order) as a measure of response intensity. The intruder species can be used as "natural models" of female conspecifics and one may compare the intensity of response to the degree of similarity to the female. Figure 3 shows the mean distance pursued for each type of intruder. If we compare these distances to a list of characteristics of each type of intruder (Table 1) we see that the degree of similarity to the female is roughly positively correlated with the intensity of response of the male. There is at least a rough relationship with response intensity (distance) for all characteristics considered, so it is difficult to determine to which the male responds and which are most impor- tant.
As with P. bicinctus the response is greatest towards the females not carrying prey. The response to females ranged from 1.4 to 4.0 m (N = 13; mean = 2.45 m). Several times it terminated with the male pouncing upon the female. The response to males averaged much less (N = 13; mean = 1.37 m; range 1.0 to 2.0 m; t-test for difference between females and males significant, p 5 .0001) but the attribute responsible for this difference was not distinctly discernable. Ap- parently some subtle difference between male and female flight characteristics is sufficient to elicit responses of different intensities
================================================================================
19791 O'Neill - Behavior of Philanthus psyche 29 by the territorial males. It is evident that the pursuit response as measured above was investigative rather than aggressive. The response becomes aggressive only if a pursued male reacts and a swirling flight ensues.
The response to females with prey is less than that to males (p 5 0.016; t-test) and females without prey. The proximate cause for this may be that the female with prey flies slower and is bulkier in appearance because of the burden she carries. An ultimate cause may be that the females with prey are not sexually receptive, having in all likelihood mated prior to or early in the sequence of nest provisioning.
Species that differ greatly in appearance and habits from female P. psyche, such as male velvet ants, spider wasps, asilid flies, butterflies and Ammophila spp. were merely approached or chased a short distance (never more than 70 cm; N = 110). In contrast, Bembecinus nanus, a sphecid wasp similar in appearance and habits to conspecific females, drew nearly the same response as females with prey (t-test, difference not significant). Coloration seems to be an important characteristic, but not the only one to which the males cue. This is evident because the bombyliid fly and the cuckoo wasp, which differ greatly in appearance from the females, usually (twenty- eight of thirty-four cases combined) drew more than a simple approach response from the males. The response to these two was significantly different (p 5 0.05) than that given to Bembixpruinosa (=B. pallidipicta) a sphecid with color patterns similar to P. psyche but of differing flight characteristics. Evidently, flight characteristics (speed and/or flight pattern) of the intruder are involved in motivating the male.
Predation Upon Males
One possible constraint upon the evolution of a mating system is the susceptibility to predation which a particular strategy imposes upon the males. There is a perceivable risk of predation by visually hunting robberflies (Asilidae) caused by the exposure of males during the large number of flights they take while on the territories. The risk is particularly high when these flights are directed towards robberflies intruding on the territory. Robberflies constituted al- most 5% of the intruders that the males approached and/ or pursued while territorial. Four attempted predations were observed, two of these successful. Predation will be discussed in more detail in another paper (Gwynne and O'Neill in press).
================================================================================
Table 1.
Species Size
Philanthus psyche Q 9 mm
Philanthus psyche 8 8 mm
Philanfhus psyche 9 mm
$ w/ Prey bulkier
Bemhecinus nanus 10 mm
(Sphecidae)
Eumenid wasp -12 mm
Microbembex spp. 10-12 mm
(S p hecidae) stouter
Bombyliid fly 8-12 mm
stouter
Similarities and differences between Philanthus psyche females and other insect intruders on the territories. Speed of Height of
Coloration flight* flight*
yellow & black w/ - -
striped abdomen
same as Q same same
same but prey may slower
affect appearance
white & black w/ same
striped abdomen
yellow, black & same
orange-red
same
same
same
same as 2
same or same
faster
black w/ white same or same
hairs faster
Flight
Volume 86 table of contents