Cambridge Entomological Club, 1874
PSYCHE

A Journal of Entomology

founded in 1874 by the Cambridge Entomological Club
Quick search

Print ISSN 0033-2615
January 2008: Psyche has a new publisher, Hindawi Publishing, and is accepting submissions

Wayne W. Tolbert.
Aerial Dispersal Behavior of Two Orb Weaving Spiders.
Psyche 84:13-27, 1977.

Full text (searchable PDF, 2288K)
Durable link: http://psyche.entclub.org/84/84-013.html


The following unprocessed text is extracted from the PDF file, and is likely to be both incomplete and full of errors. Please consult the PDF file for the complete article.

AERIAL DISPERSAL BEHAVIOR OF TWO
ORB WEAVING SPIDERS
BY WAYNE W. TOLBERT*
Graduate Program in Ecology
University of Tennessee
Knoxville, Tennessee 379 16
Introduction
Aerial dispersal, the transport of spiders from place to place by wind and/or convection currents, has been recognized as a feature of spider behavior since the time of Aristotle (Duffey, 1956). Many natural historians and arachnologists have observed and briefly commented on this phenomenon (Emerton, 1908; Bristowe, 1939; Gertsch, 1949; Nishiki, 1966; and Kaston, 1972), and a few studies have been devoted to the environmental conditions associated with the general phenomenon of spider dispersal. Most studies and observations have been made of mass migra- tions of several species of spiders, particularly migrations occur- ring during the winter months (Bristowe, 1939). Duffey (1956) determined that temperature, population density, and stages of the breeding cycle are associated with mass aerial migrations of several species of Linyphiidae. Van Wingerden and Vugts (1974) pro- duced results similar to Duffey's for one lingphiid species, Erigone artica (White).
Richter (1970, 1971) has studied in the laboratory some micro- climatic factors which influence aerial dispersal in eight species of Pardosa wolf spiders. Richter (1970) related the frequency of aero- nautic behavior of each species to the abundance and stability and that species' preferred habitat.
The purposes of this study are to describe the aerial dispersal behavior of Argiope trifasciata (Forskal), to compare behavioral elements of this species with a sympatric population of the con- gener, A. aurantia (Lucas), and to determine, under actual field conditions, the major physical parameters which influence these behaviors. Factors influencing emergence from the egg sac are discussed elsewhere (Tolbert, 1976, in preparation). *present address - Science Applications, Inc. P. 0. Box 843
Oak Ridge, Tennessee 37830
Manuscript received by the editor May 27, 1977 Pu&e Ìö4:IJ-2 (1977). hup Ytpsychu einclub orgt8.UM-Oll html



================================================================================

Psyche
[March
Methods
This study was conducted in April and May 1975, in a two hectare overgrown pasture in Loudon County, Tennessee; a conlplete de- scription of the study area is reported elsewhere (Tolbert, 1976). The study area is 3.2 kilometers (2 miles), west of Glendale com- munity. Spiderlings of two orb weaving spider species, Argiope trifasciata and Argiope aurantia, were observed after emergence from more than 50 egg sacs. The spiderlings from egg sacs were monitored for dispersal and related activities during daylight hours. Spider body temperatures were estimated by the use of a thermo- couple junction and associated cylindrical solder model of the same dimensions as a first instar spiderling (0.5 mm X 1.5 mm). Direct readout of model temperatures at dispersal height were recorded on an Esterline-Angus Continuously Recording Wind speeds at dispersal height were measured miniature cup anemometer, stall speed 0.25 m/ sec. Behavior Prior to Dispersal
Potentiometer.
with a Rimco
Unlike Argiope aurantia spiderlings, which emerge from an egg sac over a period of several days or weeks (Tolbert, 1976), Argiope trifasciata emerged in mass during the study period of April and May 1975. All spiderlings emerged from any given egg sac within a single day, usually within a period of one to two hours. This was confirmed by cutting open egg sacs from which spiderlings had recently emerged and checking for spiderlings that were left. An ethogram which summarizes the findings of this study on Argiope spp. dispersal behavior is depicted in Figure 1. Individuals quickly constructed a communal tangle or communal web, which is a mesh- work of interlocking threads, by laying down draglines. Division of labor was not observed; each spiderling simply contributed a small amount of silk to the tangle. Spiderlings observed in 1975 resided on such tangles for several days (i 31 5'. E. = 3.5 & 0.52 days) before dispersing. Valerio (1975) reports that the common house spider Achaeranea iepidorium (C. L. Koch), also spends three to four days in dense clusters on the maternal web before dispersing aerially. Communal tangle formation by Argiope au- rantia was less common. In the four instances where communal '
tangles were constructed by A. aurantia, the egg sacs had fallen to the substrate. Possibly a differing microclimate near the



================================================================================

Tolbert - Orb Weaving Spiders
SPIDERS IN EGG SAC
T
EMERGENCE
COMMUNAL WEB CONSTRUCTION
'BODY TEMP. < Xå¡C
IMMOBILITY
(WIND GUSTS, PREDATORS, ETC.)
TIP- TOE SUSPEND
BEHAVIOR ON THREAD
v
AIRBORNE
CHANGE WEB SITE "REBAHOONING*
BY WALKING
Figure 1.
An ethogram of Argiope spp. dispersal behavior.



================================================================================

16 Psyche [March
ground as demonstrated by Geiger (1965) influences communal tangle formation. Spiderlings emerging high in the vegetation ex- perience greater exposure to wind which might stimulate ballooning (discussed below). Argiope aurantia locate egg sacs higher in the vegetation than A. trfasciata. In 1974-1975 and 1975-1976 the means and standard errors of A. aurantia egg sac heights were 0.91 k 0.11 and 1.10 k 0.13 m, respectively (Tolbert, 1976). Argiope trifasciata egg sacs by comparison, were 0.30 k 0.04 m in 1975-1 976 (Tolbert, 1976).
Spiderlings on a communal tangle were tolerant of one another at all times and readily accepted conspecifics from different egg sacs. Several such "transplants" were made during the spring of 1975 with spiderlings from one egg sac transferred to the communal tangle of spiderlings from a different egg sac. After a brief flurry of activity produced by the arrival of the transplants, activity de- creased to levels noted before the introductions. At night, early in the morning, and on overcast days Argiope trifasciata spiderlings maintained individual spacing of several times their own body length while on the communal tangle (Figure 2). If disturbed by predators, wind gusts, or rain, spiderlings became agitated but quickly resumed a quiescent attitude if their body temperatures were less than 26OC. On the other hand, if exposed to full sunlight individuals clustered closely together on the com- munal tangle (Figure 3). If cues were not present for dispersal, the spiderlings remained clustered until after nightfall before spacing out. Though not timed, clustering appeared to take less than a minute. A longer period was required for clustering if the group was partially shaded. The exact cue or clues producing clustering are not known, but clustering occurred only after full exposure to bright sunlight. Air temperatures, spiderling body temperatures, light levels, and perhaps other factors which might provide the required stimuli for clustering all change at this time. More experinienta- tion is needed for elucidation of this problem. When Argiope trifasciata spiderlings are clustered and their body temperatures equalled or exceeded 26OC they were susceptible to dispersal. As indicated in Figure 4, dispersal ceased below 26OC and the preferred temperature range for dispersal was between 33O and 3Så¡C Ninety-two percent of all A. trifasciata spiderlings be- came airborne or "ballooned" when their estimated body tempera- tures were between 33' and 38OC. Argiope aurantia also dispersed



================================================================================

19771 Tolbert - Orb Weaving Spiders 17




================================================================================

18 Psyche [March




================================================================================

Tolbert - Orb Weaving Spiders
when their body temperatures were above 26' C, and their preferred range was virtually identical to that of Argiope trifasciata (Figure 5). Seventy-four percent of A. aurantia spiderlings dispersed when their body temperatures were between 33' and 38' C. A disturbance (gust of wind, striking the vegetation to which the communal tan- gle is attached, striking the tangle itself, and fanning the cluster of spiders directly) resulted in spiderling movement on the tangle. The nature of the disturbance seems to be vibrational and subsequent movement by spiderlings on the interlocking mass of silk threads of the communal tangle appeared to reinforce the initial disturb- ance. When a sufficient disturbance occurred and spider body tem- peratures were 26OC or higher, the spiderlings climbed to the top of any available object, usually vegetation. Spiderlings followed one another in mass to the top of a promontory where they pre- pared to disperse. Neither disturbance nor elevated temperature by itself was sufficient to trigger climbing behavior of A. trifasciata (Figure 1). By monitoring spiderling body temperatures on the communal tangle (spiderlings of eight Argiope trifasciata egg sacs in 1975), it was apparent that the spiderlings often experienced tem- peratures within their observed dispersal range (26' -42' C), but did not disperse. This was also evident by the time of residency on communal tangles during sunny periods (Table 1). Striking the vegetation near the tangle or fanning the cluster of spiderlings during times when body temperatures equalled or exceeded 26'C invariably induced climbing. Similar stimulation at body temper- atures of less than 26' C failed to produce climbing behavior. In April 1976, two additional clusters of A. trifasciata spiderlings were tested. Even when body temperatures exceeded 26OC and a vibratory stimulus was applied, these spiderlings could not be in- duced to climb and disperse. While these findings might simply be abberations or due to some genetic differences between populations between the two years, or to some microclimatic variables which were not examined, it seems likely that developmental differences between the spiderlings which emerged in May and those in April may account for the observed difference in behavior. A. trifasciata overwintered as eggs and the active spiderlings did not develop until April (Tolbert, 1976). A difference of as much as one month to six weeks in the developmental age of spiderlings could influence behavior. It is possible that the older spiderlings would be more prone to dispersal behavior than younger spider-



================================================================================

Psyche
[March
26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 SPIDER BODY (MODEL) TEMPERATURE PC)
Figure 4.
Number of Argiope zrifasciata ballooning as a function of body (model) temperature.
lings. Residence time by spiderlings on communal tangles was somewhat longer in early May than in late May (Table 1). Com- puting mean residence time (in days) from the data in Table 1, re- veals 7.5 days for the spiderlings emerging on 10 May, 8 days for 11 May, 5 days for 14 May, 2.3 days for 19 May, and 4 days for 22 May.
Richter (1970) has stated that dispersal by spiders normally oc- curs on days that are unusually warm and calm for the time of year. In the same study, he demonstrated that a wolf spider, Par- dosa purbeckenis F. 0. B. Cambridge exhibited aeronautic be- havior when laboratory air temperatures were varied between 18' and 34' C.
The percentages of aeronautic behavior for each tem- perature range he used (which I calculated from his Table 4) were 6.9% (18-19' C), 34.8% (28-29' C) and 36.4% (33-34' C). In 1975, 20 of the 24 A. trifasciata spiderling masses dispersed on sunny days (see Table 1). Thus, it appears that dispersal in Argiope is influenced by climatogical factors, and in particular temperature. In addition to these findings, Duffey (1966) in monitoring winter



================================================================================

Tolbert - Orb Weaving Spiders
26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 SPIDER BODY (MODEL) TEMPERATURE (OC)
Figure 5.
Number of Argiope aurantia ballooning as a function of body (model) temperature.
dispersal of linyphiids, indicated that increasing litter temperatures were partially responsible for increased aerial dispersal by mem- bers of this litter-dwelling spider family. Gypsy moth (Porthetria dispar L.) larvae ascend trees preparatory to aerial dispersal when ambient air temperatures are between 15.6OC (50å¡F and 29.4OC (85O F). These larvae, however, have black dorsal surfaces and their body temperatures when exposed to direct sunlight can easily ex- ceed air temperatures (McManus, 1973). Thus, the aerial dispersal of some spider and insect species is influenced by temperature. Aeronautic Behavior
In preparation for dispersal, individuals may adopt either a "tip- toe" posture, as defined by Richter (1970) or hang suspended from a dragline from which they become airborne (Figure 1). he tip- toe stance, which is widely employed by lycosid spiders (Richter, 1970), results when the spider depresses the cephalothorax toward the substrate and elevates the abdomen. Silk lines are then exuded from the spinnerets. Multiple lines of ballooning silk were often



================================================================================

22 Psyche [March
Table 1.
Argiope trfasciaia residency on communal tangles and associated weather conditions during May, 1975. S = sunny, PC = partly cloudy, C = cloudy, X = presence, Y = dispersal on same day as emergence. Dispersal occurred on last day spiderlings were present.
Weather S S P C S S C C C S S S S SPCPCS Egg sac
number
x x x x x x
x x x x x x x
x x x x x x x x x
x x x x x x
x x x x x x x x x
x x x x x x x x
x x x x x x x x
x x x x x x x x
x x x x x
x x
x x
x x
Y
x x x x
x x
x x
x x x x
Y
X X X
X X X
X X X
X X X
x x
x x x x
observed and there appeared to be four to six lines, but the exact number was not determined. Spiderlings also dispersed directly from silk threads. They dropped on a dragline, cut it and while suspended and holding this thread by leg pairs I and 11, let out ballooning silk as before, until they became airborne. Spiderlings, as a rule, became airborne during relatively calm periods with a preferred windspeed for dispersal of approximately 0.5 m/sec. (Figure 6). During gusty periods spiderlings either did not exhibit tip-toe postures at all or if in that posture, returned to



================================================================================

Tolbert - Orb Weaving Spiders
WIND SPEED (meters/second)
Figure 6.
Number of Argiope trifasciata ballooning as a function of windspeed. a normal resting attitude. Spiderlings did not disperse at wind- speeds of greater than 0.9 m/ sec. (Figure 6). Richter (1971) showed that Pardosa purbeckensis had preferred windspeeds for dispersal which were related to their body sizes; larger spiders preferred greater windspeeds. All size classes, however, preferred wind veloc- ities between 0.35 and 1.70 m/sec. These values exceed those ob- served for Argiope trifasciata, which is a slightly larger spider than Pardosa purbeckensis. From their study of spiders on Frisian Is- land (the Netherlands) Van Wingerden and Vugts (1974) concluded that aeronautic behavior ceased when the wind velocity at 2 m above the substrate exceeded 3.0 m/sec. They also found in the same study that unstable air masses near the ground and at dispersal height served as stimuli to aeronautic behaviour. Disturbances (wind gusts) apparently function in a similar fashion for Argiope trifasciata.
After a spiderling becomes airborne it will travel on wind cur- rents until deposited at a potential web location. The spiderling may then build a web, or move a short distance from the deposi- tion site (perhaps searching for a site of better potential for web construction), and then build a web (Figure 1). It might also re- balloon with or without "searching behavior." Data on the relative



================================================================================

Psyche
[March
frequencies of these behaviors are scarce. Approximately 20 argio- pids were observed to reballoon during the course of the study, some as many as six times with the majority (90%) of these observed reballooning bouts occurring when the spiderlings were in close proximity to the egg sac. It is possible that a minimum time or distance requirement must be achieved before the spiderling has satisfied a "ballooning drive." It might also reflect investigator error since spiderlings which drop into dense vegetation are diffi- cult to detect and the probability of finding individuals must de- crease with distance from the egg sac (with decreased density of spiderlings). Nevertheless, multiple ballooning bouts probably func- tion to increase emigration from a given area (which would tend to lessen competition for food, web sites or other resources if such resources are in short supply). Since Argiope spp. actively select web sites (Enders, 1972, 1973), it is probable that areas judged un- suitable by the spiderlings can be quickly and easily abandoned via reballooning. This finding is consistent with the predictions of Doyle's (1975) habitat selection model. Organisms which encounter coarse-grained (patchy) environments, as Argiope do, can improve fitness by selecting habitat types yielding highest survivorship. Riech- ert (1973) demonstrated that a desert agelenid spider, Agelenopsis aperta (Gertsch) improved the quality of its web sites by successive relocation in better web sites. A. aperta walked rather than bal- looned to new web sites, however. Riechert and Tracy (1975) pro- duced a model of reproductive success that demonstrates a thirteen fold advantage in fecundity of spiders living in good versus poor web sites. Habitat selection and differential survival of Argiope spp. is an area worthy of additional research. One or more days may elapse after the spiderling locates a web site before it actually builds a web there. This is based on two sets of observations. First, spiderlings discovered in the field without webs and checked later the same day had not built webs (N = 5). Only spiderlings found early in the morning and thus not recently dispersed are considered here. Two of these individuals built webs one day later; one built two days later. Second, even during the height of Argiope trifasciata aerial dispersal (mid-May) few spider- lings were observed to have webs. Possibly the spiderlings were adjusting from a colonial, passive existence to one of active preda- tion. Whatever the reason(s), considerable mortality is suffered during this period (Tolbert, 1976). Spiderlings would certainly be



================================================================================

19771 Tolbert - Orb Weaving Spiders 25
vulnerable to predation, especially since they do not have the pro- tection a web affords (Tolbert, 1975). Some spiderlings probably starve during this time also (Tolbert, 1976). Spiderlings underwent a radical change in behavior toward con- specifics after dispersing from the communal tangle. As long as the spiderlings were on the communal tangle they were completely tolerant of one another and duringhot sunny periods clustered very closely together. However, after a spiderling constructed an orb web it attacked and ate any prey that contacted the web. Litter-
mates were invariably attacked and treated as prey items when they encountered or were placed on conspecific's orb web. However, it is unnecessary for spiderlings of either Argiope species to engage in aerial dispersal before building an orb web. I have successfully
reared both species in the laboratory after removing them from egg sacs. After building their first orb web and without the benefit of living on or building a communal tangle or engaging in aerial dispersal, Argiope spiderlings still attacked and killed littermates placed on their orb webs.
Although Enders (1972) indicated he has observed unsuccessful ballooning attempts by mid-instar Argiope aurantia, my observa- tions indicate that only the emergent (first instar) disperses aerially. Summary
Although both species are capable of all the behaviors in the ethogram (Figure l), Argiope aurantia, possibly by virtue of their higher, more exposed egg sac locations, generally dispersed shortly after emergence from the egg sac. Argiope trifasciata produced communal tangles and engaged in preaeronautic behaviors on these structures before dispersing.
In 1975, both spider species ballooned when their estimated body temperature exceeded 26' C, with most dispersal occurring when body temperatures were between 33' and 38' C. A vibrational stim- ulus occurring when spider body temperatures are above 26OC re- sulted in climbing behavior by the mass spiderlings on the com- munal tangle. Upon reaching the top of a promontory they either became airborne from that point or dropped on a dragline and ballooned from that position. Spiderlings ballooned at relatively low wind speeds (0.5 m/s). Multiple ballooning bouts were ob- served for some individuals. It is hypothesized that aerial dispersal serves to rapidly space spiderlings over available habitats such that



================================================================================

26 Psyche [March
overcrowding is minimized. Aerial dispersal would also allow suc- cessional species such as A. trifasciata and A. aurantia to colonize newly opened, ephemeral habitats.
I gratefully acknowledge the assistance of James Tanner, Gordon Burghardt, Charles Pless and David Etnier who ably served on my doctoral committee. I am especially grateful to Susan Riechert, my committee chairperson. I acknowledge the financial support of the National Science Foundation, Grant For Doctoral Disserta- tion Improvement (BMS 74-17602) and the Graduate Program in Ecology of the University of Tennessee.
BRISTOWE, W. S.
1939.
The comity of spiders, Vol. I. Johnson Reprint Corp., New York. DOYLE, R. W.
1975.
Settlement of planktonic larvae: A theory of habitat selection in vary- ing environments. Amer. Natur. 109: 113-126. DUFFEY, E.
1956.
Aerial dispersal in a known spider population. J. Anim. Ecol. 25: 85-111.
ENDERS, F.
1972.
Web-site selection by Argiope aurantia Lucas and other orb weaving spiders (Araneidae). Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, N. C. State Uni- versity.
ENDERS, F.


Volume 84 table of contents